Skip to main content

Pre-scan state anxiety is associated with greater right amygdala-hippocampal response to fearful versus happy faces among trait-anxious Latina girls

Abstract

Background

Unfamiliarity with academic research may contribute to higher levels of anticipatory state anxiety about affective neuroimaging tasks. Children with high trait anxiety display differences in brain response to fearful facial affect compared to non-anxious youth, but little is known about the influence of state anxiety on this association. Because reduced engagement in scientific research and greater mistrust among minoritized groups may lead to systematic differences in pre-scan state anxiety, it is crucial to understand the neural correlates of state anxiety during emotion processing so as to disambiguate sources of individual differences.

Methods

The present study probed the interactive effects of pre-scan state anxiety, trait anxiety, and emotional valence (fearful vs. happy faces) on neural activation during implicit emotion processing in a community sample of 46 preadolescent Latina girls (8–13 years).

Results

Among girls with mean and high levels of trait anxiety, pre-scan state anxiety was associated with greater right amygdala-hippocampal and left inferior parietal lobe response to fearful faces relative to happy faces.

Conclusions

Anticipatory state anxiety in the scanning context may cause children with moderate and high trait anxiety to be hypervigilant to threats, further compounding the effects of trait anxiety. Neuroimaging researchers should control for state anxiety so that systematic differences in brain activation resulting from MRI apprehension are not misleadingly attributed to demographic or environmental characteristics.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Accurate recognition of others’ emotional expressions provides us with cues to salient environmental features and the emotional state of our interaction partners. Children with anxiety exhibit threat biases in processing and interpreting facial affect [1,2,3,4] and display atypical patterns of neural activation when viewing threat stimuli [5]. Perceiving social cues as threatening may cause anxious children to avoid social situations, reducing opportunities to habituate or reappraise fears and further exacerbating anxiety symptoms [6]. Momentary feelings of anxiety in healthy children, or state anxiety, can also elicit behavioral and neural responses to negative emotional stimuli resembling those seen in trait anxiety and anxiety disorders [7,8,9,10]. Many children experience elevated state anxiety while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning [11]. However, few developmental studies on anxiety test how pre-scan state anxiety influences neural substrates of emotion processing, so it is unknown whether previous findings capture trait features of anxiety or state features associated with apprehension of the neuroimaging environment or both.

Given that some demographic groups may experience greater state anxiety about scanning due to limited experience with research or medical mistrust [12,13,14], it is critical to understand the neural correlates of elevated state anxiety during emotion processing, and whether they are distinct from, or overlapping with those seen in trait anxiety. In the present study, a community sample of Latina girls (8–13 years) completed an fMRI implicit emotion processing task, during which they viewed fearful and happy faces varying in emotion intensity and reported the face’s gender. We tested the interactive effects of state anxiety, trait anxiety, and emotional valence on girls’ neural responses to facial affect.

Anxious children display increased threat vigilance to fearful and angry facial affect relative to non-anxious children [1, 3, 4, 15, 16] and are more likely to appraise emotional stimuli as negative or threatening [2, 17,18,19]. Several brain networks are associated with threat biases in children and adults with anxiety disorders. Children and adults with anxiety disorders display elevated activation in the amygdala and insula to negative or ambiguous facial affect [20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29], atypical recruitment of prefrontal, executive control networks [30,31,32,33,34], and reduced connectivity between these networks [35,36,37,38,39,40,41]. The amygdala and insula are highly interconnected structures that contribute to the peripheral expression of emotion and salience detection [42,43,44]. Upon viewing a threat, the amygdala signals the production of a threat response and increases vigilance [45]. The insula is involved in interoception and plays a crucial role in subjective emotions [46, 47]. Given these regions’ central role in identifying a stimulus’ emotional significance and generating an affective response [48, 49], they are hypothesized to be important for processing negative and positive affective information [50]. Thus, amygdala and insula hyperactivity may contribute to anxiety symptoms, such that negative social information is assigned greater salience in anxious than in non-anxious individuals.

The MRI context itself can elicit temporary feelings of state anxiety, discomfort, or even panic among children [11, 51], which may evoke patterns of activation that are unique from [52], or overlapping with, trait anxiety and anxiety disorders [53]. State anxiety in the fMRI context is an essential consideration in pediatric anxiety research, but, to date, it is understudied (see Michalska et al. (2020) [54] for a detailed review of methodological considerations and challenges of the scanning environment). Children undergoing fMRI scans are often alone in the confined space in the scanner bore, where they must tolerate loud noises and restricted motion [55]. This experience can elicit physical discomfort [56] and anxiety [11, 51] and increase biological indices of stress like cortisol [57, 58]. Elevated state anxiety can impact task performance [59, 60] and influence attentional [61, 62], perceptual [63,64,65,66], and interpretative mechanisms [59, 67, 68]. State anxiety also elicits changes in blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response while participants view threatening or emotional stimuli [7,8,9,10] and even during rest [52, 69]. Importantly, although undergoing MRI scanning induces stress in about 30% of participants [11], few studies test how pre-scan state anxiety impacts subsequent task performance. Further, although state anxiety can interact with trait anxiety to predict behavioral responses to negative emotional stimuli [60, 70, 71], little is known about how state and trait anxiety interact to predict brain activation during affective neuroimaging tasks.

Undergoing an MRI scan is anxiety-inducing, not just for children [51, 72], but also more generally for people unfamiliar with the scanning environment [73, 74]. Because minoritized groups like Latinx participants are underrepresented in research and, for historic reasons, display greater mistrust in medical, academic, and scientific institutions than white participants [12,13,14], there may be systematic differences in state anxiety across demographic groups that lead to inaccurate interpretations of results. For instance, higher rates of pre-scan state anxiety among a minoritized group in a study may lead to greater alterations in emotion processing, cognitive functioning, or physiology in that group [75]. Without accounting for state anxiety, such task-related differences could be misattributed to temperamental, environmental, or cultural factors rather than apprehension of the research environment.

In the present study, a community sample of predominantly Mexican American girls (8–13 years) completed an implicit face emotion viewing task while in an MRI scanner. During this task, children viewed graded levels of happy and fearful faces varying in emotion intensity and reported on the face’s gender. We tested the interactive effects of trait anxiety and state anxiety on mean neural response to emotional facial affect. To evaluate whether interactive effects of state and trait anxiety were specific to threatening facial affect, we also tested whether emotional valence (fearful vs. happy) moderated any observed associations. We hypothesized that both state and trait anxiety would be associated with increased amygdala and insula response to fearful facial affect [20, 21, 23, 76]. Following prior work showing that state anxiety increases attentional and interpretive threat biases only for trait-anxious people [60, 70], we also hypothesized that state and trait anxiety would interact to predict amygdala and insula response to fearful affect, such that state anxiety would increase activation among trait-anxious youth. As preliminary work observes trending associations between childhood anxiety and amygdala response to positive social cues [77, 78], we did not have specific predictions about the effects of state or trait anxiety on happy facial affect. Instead, we hypothesized that the effects of anxiety on activation in salience processing regions would be greater for fearful than happy facial affect.

Methods

Participants and procedure

Fifty-five 8-13-year-old Latina girls and their primary caregivers were recruited from the Inland Empire of Southern California to participate in a longitudinal study of emotional development. Participants were recruited via the Psychology Department’s shared database of child participants recruited from the community. Participant eligibility was determined by phone screening with the primary caregiver. Children were eligible for participation if they were between 8 and 13 years old, proficient in English, right-handed, and had no contraindications for neuroimaging (e.g., no ferrous metal in the body, not pregnant, not claustrophobic). Children also needed to be at least 50% Latinx origin and self-identify as Latina to be eligible for participation (see Table 1 for ethnic-racial identity of participants in the final sample). Exclusionary criteria included a current psychiatric diagnosis of Tourette’s syndrome, obsessive-compulsive disorder, lifetime history of mania, psychosis, or pervasive developmental disorder. Menstruation onset was initially used as an exclusionary criterion but was dropped to increase sample size, and two postmenarchal participants were recruited.

Table 1 Racial and ethnic background of study participants (N = 46)

Participants completed a laboratory testing session and a scanning session. During the laboratory session, children and caregivers reported on family demographics and children’s behavior, anxiety, and other mental health outcome measures not reported here. During the scanning session, children completed an implicit face emotion viewing task while undergoing fMRI data collection. fMRI scans were not collected from seven participants because they did not return for the scan visit (n = 4), or due to participants’ distress (n = 1), dental braces (n = 1), or experimenter error (n = 1). Two participants were excluded due to low response rate on the task (> 25% missed trials), resulting in a final sample of 46 participants (Mage = 9.9 ± 1.2 years; Table 2) and their caregivers (40 mothers, 6 fathers). The visit structure was changed part-way through data collection, so 11 participants completed the laboratory session and scanning session at two visits, approximately two weeks apart (M = 19.7 days, + 4.8). The remaining 35 participants completed the laboratory and scanning sessions in one visit. Results were largely unchanged when days elapsed between visits was added as a covariate (Table S1). Upon participant arrival at each wave, written parent consent and child assent were obtained. At the end of each session, participants were compensated with a gift card and a toy. All study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board. All data were collected prior to the COVID-19 pandemic.

Table 2 Sample demographic characteristics and descriptive statistics for study variables

Measures

Demographic characteristics

Psychological research is commonly conducted among white, educated, upper- and middle-class samples [79]. Thus, communities with lower income or less access to education may have less exposure to, and more apprehension of, scientific research. We tested whether state anxiety was associated with individual differences in household income, parental education, and children’s perceived social standing relative to their community and the United States. As our sample was fairly homogenous in ethnicity and city of residence, we did not test for associations of state anxiety with these variables.

Parents indicated their child’s age and ethnic background, as well as their own educational background and household income. We also measured children’s self-reported subjective socioeconomic status using a modified version of the MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status [80]. This two-question measure probed individuals’ perceptions of where they rank in the status hierarchy of (1) their community, and (2) the United States. Participants ranked their status by indicating the rung on a nine-rung ladder, on which the top of the ladder represented “people who are the best off, those who have the most money, most education, and best jobs” and the bottom represented “people who are the worst off, those who have the least money, least education, worst jobs, or no job.”

State and trait anxiety symptoms

Children’s state and trait anxiety symptoms were measured via child self-report on the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAIC) [81] (Fig. 1). The STAIC is comprised of two 20-item scales that assess state anxiety (STAIC-State) and trait anxiety (STAIC-Trait). Children respond to all items on a three-point Likert scale, and each subscale is summed to a total score (range: 20–60). Both measures display excellent internal consistency (α 0.81) [82]. The STAIC-State measures state anxiety by asking children to indicate how they feel “right now…at this moment.” State anxiety was measured in the imaging facility immediately before the scan. The STAIC-State was added to the protocol shortly after data collection began, so three participants did not complete it and their scores were imputed with mean-replacement. The STAIC-Trait assesses trait levels of anxiety by probing how the child usually feels. Children’s trait anxiety was measured in the lab, prior to the scan. The STAIC-Trait demonstrates concurrent validity with other anxiety measures (r = .88) [82]. In our sample, the STAIC-Trait was highly correlated with child report on the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders, r = .81, p < .001 (SCARED) [83], which assesses anxiety disorder symptomatology. State and trait anxiety scores on the STAIC were uncorrelated in our sample, r = .04, p = .77.

Although our sample was a non-treatment-seeking community sample, children’s self-reported anxiety scores were notably elevated, according to their self-report on the SCARED (Table 2). The mean score was 38.6, and more than 80% of participants surpassed the threshold for clinically significant anxiety levels ( 25; N = 38). These scores are substantially higher than those self-reported by clinically anxious girls and boys (7–18 years; MSCARED = 23.8) [84] and female psychiatric outpatients (6–17 years; MSCARED = 25.8) [85].

As mentioned above, due to the change in visit structure mid-way through data collection, 11 participants reported trait and state anxiety at separate visits, approximately two weeks apart (M = 19.7 days, ± 4.8). Post hoc sensitivity analyses were conducted to include the time elapsed between state and trait anxiety collection, which had minor influences on reported effects (see Supplement).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Scatterplot and density distributions for participants’ state and trait anxiety. Note: State and trait anxiety were assessed via the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (Spielberger et al., 1973); SD = standard deviation

Implicit face emotion viewing task

While undergoing fMRI scanning, children completed an implicit face emotion viewing task [86, 87], during which they labeled the gender of ten actors’ face emotion pictures (100% white; 60% female). Faces were morphed between neutral and fearful or happy expressions at 6%, 30%, 54%, and 78% emotion intensity (Fig. 2). Faces were presented in random order for 2000 ms each, followed by a 500–3000 ms jittered interstimulus interval (ISI) during which a white fixation cross was presented against a black background. In one run, children viewed 20 trials of each morphed fearful and happy stimulus, summing to 160 trials total and 80 trials of fear morphs. The task was programmed in E-prime (version 2.0.10; PST Inc., Pittsburgh, PA). Participants viewed the back-projected screen via a mirror mounted on the head coil and pressed a button box with their right hand to indicate the gender of the face (male/female).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Implicit emotion viewing paradigm with happy and fearful facial stimuli. Participants viewed black and white faces that were morphed blends between neutral and fearful or happy emotional expressions at 6%, 30%, 54%, and 78% emotion intensities. Participants responded with the gender of the face. Faces were presented in random order for 2000 ms each, followed by a 500-3000 ms jittered interstimulus interval

Imaging data

MRI data acquisition and preprocessing

Whole-brain neuroimaging data were collected using a 3T Siemens Prisma scanner and 32-channel head coil. Two hundred and forty functional image volumes were collected during one 9 min 44 s run. Functional image volumes with 62 contiguous interleaved axial slices were obtained with a T2*-weighted echo-planar sequence (TR = 2500 ms; TE = 32 ms; flip angle = 80; Field of View [FOV] = 204 × 228 mm; matrix = 102 × 114; voxel size = 2 × 2 × 2 mm3). Using a magnetization-prepared gradient echo sequence, functional data were anatomically localized and coregistered to a high-resolution T1-weighted volumetric scan of the whole brain that was collected prior to the functional volumes (MPRAGE: TR = 2400 ms; TE = 2.72 ms; TI = 1060 ms; flip angle = 8; FOV = 240 × 256 mm; matrix = 300 × 320; voxel size = 0.8 x 0.8 x 0.8 mm3).

Individual echo-planar images were preprocessed and analyzed using AFNI (Analysis of Functional NeuroImages; version 22.0; Cox, 1996 [88]). Preprocessing included despiking, slicetime correction, motion correction, and smoothing with a 4 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) kernel. All MRI data were transformed to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. BOLD data was scaled at the voxel-wise time series by their temporal means so effect estimates can be interpreted as percent signal change. Every TR on which motion exceeded 2 mm was censored, and excessive motion was defined as more than 20% of TRs censored for motion/outliers (N = 0). One participant had 20.4% of TRs censored and was included in all analyses. Average head motion was not correlated with state anxiety (r = .15, p = .30) or trait anxiety (r = .24, p = .10). Using the AFNI 3dDeconvolve function, a general linear model was generated to estimate mean task-related activation for happy and fearful facial affect, averaged across all emotion intensities. Third-order Legendre polynomials modeled baseline drift and six head motion parameters.

Statistical analyses

Behavioral analyses

Task performance was assessed via gender labeling accuracy, percent of trials participants responded to, and mean reaction time. Children’s state and trait anxiety levels on the STAIC were correlated with each behavioral measure.

Correlations were also tested between state anxiety and household income, parental education, and children’s perceived social standing relative to their community and the United States. Bonferroni correction was conducted to correct for multiple comparisons across the four demographic measures at p < .0125.

Neuroimaging analyses

To correct for multiple comparisons, familywise error correction was performed using Monte Carlo simulation on gray matter-masked, whole-brain data (3dClustSim in AFNI). The gray matter mask was created by segmenting the MNI152_2009c anatomical template into gray matter and non-gray matter. Masked output maps included gray matter voxels of the whole brain. The voxel threshold of p < .005 resulted in an average cluster threshold of 50 voxels at the whole-brain corrected alpha level of 0.05. Peak coordinates (x, y, z) are reported based on the MNI atlas in left, posterior, inferior (LPI) orientation.

A linear mixed-effects model was conducted using AFNI’s 3dLME program [89]. The model tested the independent and interactive effects of state anxiety, trait anxiety, and emotional valence in predicting mean task-related activation to fearful and happy faces, averaged across all intensity levels. Gray matter-masked, whole-brain voxel-wise tests were used for all fMRI analyses. Age and motion were included as covariates of no interest. Given our focus on neural responses to fear, in clusters with significant three-way interactions, we subtracted the mean neural response to happy facial affect from the mean neural response to fearful facial affect to capture BOLD response to fearful relative to happy facial affect. State anxiety was plotted on the x-axis, difference in average brain activation (BOLDFear - BOLDHappy) was plotted on the y-axis (Fig. 3). Follow-up simple slopes analyses tested the model-predicted slope for children with high (+ 1 SD), mean, and low (-1 SD) trait anxiety levels. In clusters with significant State Anxiety x Trait Anxiety interactions, average brain activation was calculated, collapsed across fearful and happy facial affect. Follow-up simple slopes analyses tested the model-predicted slope for the association between state anxiety and mean neural activation for children with high (+ 1 SD), moderate (mean), and low (-1 SD) trait anxiety levels.

Results

Behavior

Two participants responded to fewer than 75% of trials and were excluded from the final analyses with N = 46. Overall, children showed high task engagement (M = 94.6% response rate ± 5.2%) and were accurate at labeling the gender of faces (M = 91.2% ± 8.2%). State anxiety was inversely associated with average response rate across all fear trials, r = − .31, p = .039, and happy trials, r = − .32, p = .028, such that participants with greater state anxiety responded to fewer trials. Trait anxiety was not associated with response rate for fear or happy trials (ps > 0.27). Gender labeling accuracy was not correlated with state anxiety or trait anxiety for fear or happy trials (ps > 0.06). Average response time (M = 1075.0 ms ± 120.6 ms) was also unrelated to state anxiety and trait anxiety (ps > 0.50).

Post hoc linear regression analyses were also conducted to examine whether state and trait anxiety interacted to predict behavioral outcomes, controlling for age. State and trait anxiety did not interact to predict accuracy, average response time, or response rate (all ps > 0.21).

Effects of demographic characteristics on pre-scan state anxiety

Children’s pre-scan state anxiety was inversely correlated with the community subscale of the MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status, r = − .33, p = .023. In other words, children who rated themselves as lower in social standing relative to their community tended to have greater state anxiety prior to the MRI scan. However, this result did not hold after Bonferroni correction. Pre-scan state anxiety was not associated with how children rated themselves relative to people in the United States, r = − .16, p = .30. Children’s pre-scan state anxiety was also not associated with objective demographic variables, including parental education or household income, ps > 0.48.

Brain activation

State Anxiety x Trait Anxiety x Emotional Valence

State and trait anxiety interacted with stimuli’s emotional valence to predict mean BOLD response in two clusters. The first cluster encompassed portions of the right amygdala and hippocampus (k = 107, x = 27, y = -15, z = -23), and the second cluster was in the left inferior parietal lobe (IPL: k = 90, x = -57, y = -21, z = 35). Differential neural responses to fearful versus happy faces were calculated by subtracting mean neural responses to happy expressions from mean responses to fearful expressions, within each significant cluster. Follow-up simple slopes were conducted in each cluster to test the model-predicted association between state anxiety and differential neural response to fearful versus happy faces (BOLDFear - BOLDHappy) for children at low (-1 SD: 31.2), mean (38.4), and high levels of trait anxiety (+ 1 SD: 45.7).

In the right amygdala-hippocampal complex, simple slopes revealed a positive association between state anxiety and differential neural responses to fearful versus happy faces (BOLDFear - BOLDHappy) for children with mean, β = 0.01, SE = 0.002, t = 2.83, p = .007, and high levels of trait anxiety (+ 1 SD), β = 0.02, SE = 0.004, t = 5.43, p < .001 (Fig. 3a). In other words, with increasing state anxiety, children with mean and high trait anxiety displayed greater responses to fearful relative to happy facial expressions in the amygdala-hippocampal complex. By contrast, children with low trait anxiety (-1 SD) displayed an inverse association between state anxiety and differential neural response (BOLDFear - BOLDHappy), β = − 0.01, SE = 0.003, t = -2.25, p = .030, such that state anxiety was associated with decreased neural responses to fearful relative to happy facial affect.

In the left IPL, simple slopes revealed a positive association between state anxiety and differential neural responses to fearful versus happy facial affect (BOLDFear - BOLDHappy) for children with mean, β = 0.01, SE = 0.003, t = 3.33, p = .002, and high levels of trait anxiety (+ 1 SD), β = 0.02, SE = 0.004, t = 4.66, p < .001 (Fig. 3b). As with the right amygdala-hippocampal complex, with increasing state anxiety, children with mean and high trait anxiety displayed greater left IPL responses to fearful relative to happy facial affect in left IPL. No such associations emerged for children with low trait anxiety (-1 SD), β = − 0.002, SE = 0.004, t = -0.64, p = .53.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Trait anxiety moderated associations between state anxiety and neural responses to fearful versus happy faces. Results of the gray matter-masked, whole-brain linear mixed effects model. Trait anxiety moderated associations between state anxiety and differential neural responses to fearful versus happy faces (BOLDFear – BOLDHappy) in the (A) right amygdala-hippocampal complex, and (B) left inferior parietal lobe. Simple slopes depict the association between state anxiety and mean neural activation for children with low (-1 SD; STAIC-Trait = 31.2), mean (STAIC-Trait = 38.4), or high (+ 1 SD; STAIC-Trait = 45.7) trait anxiety. STAIC = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children. Groups displaying slopes that significantly differ from 0 are indicated on the legend: ***p .001, **p .01, *p .05

Independent and interactive effects of state and trait anxiety

A two-way interaction emerged between state and trait anxiety in predicting mean BOLD response in the right caudate (k = 90, x = 19, y = 19, z = 7), averaged across fearful and happy facial affect. Simple slopes tested the model-predicted association between state anxiety and mean BOLD response to all emotional affect for children with low (-1 SD: 31.2), mean (38.4), and high trait anxiety (+ 1 SD: 45.7). We observed an inverse association between state anxiety and mean neural response to emotional affect for children with mean, β = − 0.02, t = -3.12, p = .003, and high trait anxiety (+ 1 SD), β = − 0.04, t = -5.14, p < .001. State anxiety was not associated with mean right caudate activation at low levels of trait anxiety (- 1 SD), β = 0.01, t = 1.47, p = .15.

We also observed main effects of state anxiety, trait anxiety, and emotional valence. State anxiety was inversely associated with right caudate response to emotional affect (k = 141, x = 11, y = 19, z = -3). Trait anxiety was inversely associated with activation in the right inferior temporal gyrus (k = 76, x = 53, y = -25, z = -23). There was also a main effect of emotional valence in a cluster spanning portions of the right amygdala and hippocampus (k = 89, x = 23, y = -5, z = -23), such that the cluster was more reactive to fearful than happy faces.

Multivariate outlier detection

A post hoc multivariate outlier detection analysis was performed to identify outliers. The Mahalanobis distance was calculated using participants’ state anxiety scores, trait anxiety scores, and mean activation to fearful and happy facial affect within each significant cluster. No participants had a Mahalanobis distance exceeding the χ2(14) critical of 39.25 at p < .001, and all data was retained.

Discussion

Youth unfamiliar with academic and biobehavioral research settings may experience elevated levels of state anxiety in anticipation of affective neuroimaging tasks, complicating inferences. The present study probed the independent and interactive effects of state anxiety, trait anxiety, and emotional valence on neural activation during implicit emotion processing in a community sample of preadolescent Latina girls with elevated trait anxiety. State anxiety was associated with children’s subjective social status (prior to multiple comparison correction) but was uncorrelated with trait anxiety. Neuroimaging analyses revealed a three-way interaction between state anxiety, trait anxiety, and emotional valence in the right amygdala-hippocampal complex and left IPL. For children with mean and high levels of trait anxiety, state anxiety was associated with greater activation to fearful relative to happy facial affect in both clusters.

Although our sample was a non-treatment-seeking community sample, we observed high levels of self-reported trait anxiety. Over 80% of participants met criteria for clinically significant anxiety on the SCARED, underscoring the importance of community-informed anxiety research focused on Latinx youth [90]. This observation is in line with other research finding high rates of anxiety in Latinx youth [91,92,93], and Latina girls specifically [94], relative to other ethnic groups. Although this is an understudied issue, some research suggests that discriminatory experiences [95], acculturative stress [96], cultural factors [97], or parenting [98] may contribute to elevated anxiety.

Somewhat surprisingly, trait and state anxiety subscales on the STAIC were uncorrelated in our sample. State and trait anxiety are sometimes correlated in research settings [99] but not always [52]. This variability is likely because state anxiety measurements are closely tied to the specific context in which they are collected. Certain stimuli, such as scary movies, might induce anxiety in many children, while others might be more situation-specific, such as fear of flying, dentists, or anticipating a brain scan. Preliminary research suggests that state and trait anxiety have unique patterns of neural activation at rest [52], reinforcing the notion that they are distinct constructs [100] (though see [53], which finds induced anxiety parallels the effects of pathological anxiety in the insula and medial prefrontal cortex). Hence, these results suggest that the MRI setting may selectively elicit anxiety in certain youth. Moreover, other factors like unfamiliarity with the scanning environment [73, 74] and distrust of medical or scientific institutions [12,13,14] may contribute to systematic variations in state anxiety across demographic groups [101]. Psychological research is often conducted in white, educated, and affluent communities [79]. People with lower income and/or less access to education may have limited exposure to scientific research, contributing to discomfort or mistrust. Unfortunately, we did not collect measures that allowed us to directly test whether discomfort in or mistrust of the research environment specifically contributed to greater state anxiety. However, our study revealed an inverse association between state anxiety and subjective social status (prior to multiple comparison correction), such that children who rated their family as having a lower standing in the community tended to have higher pre-scan state anxiety. State anxiety was not associated with objective measures of socioeconomic status (e.g., household income, parental education), nor with children’s subjective assessment of their family’s status compared to the broader United States. These findings suggest that the scanning environment may be particularly anxiety-inducing for participants who feel marginalized relative to other members of their community. It is important to note that our study exclusively involved Latina girls residing in the Inland Empire, resulting in a relatively homogenous sample. Future research should validate this hypothesis among participants representing diverse socioeconomic, educational, and ethnic backgrounds, and directly probe participants’ experience with and trust in neuroimaging or research settings.

Neuroimaging analyses revealed that state and trait anxiety interacted with emotional valence to predict average neural activity in a cluster encompassing portions of the right amygdala-hippocampal complex. Our amygdala hypotheses were partially confirmed, such that for moderately and highly trait-anxious children, state anxiety was associated with greater right amygdala-hippocampal activity for fearful faces, compared to happy faces. The amygdala and hippocampus are highly interconnected limbic structures that are involved in processing emotional stimuli [102,103,104]. The amygdala is responsible for bottom-up, automatic threat detection and emotional arousal [29, 105,106,107]. The hippocampus facilitates emotion recognition and interpretation by contextualizing sensory input within emotional memories [102, 103, 108]. Thus, elevated amygdala-hippocampal activity can reflect increased vigilance or emotional response to threat stimuli. Both state and trait anxiety have been independently linked to elevated activation in the amygdala [7, 8, 22, 23] and hippocampus [109,110,111] in response to threatening facial affect. However, to our knowledge, this is the first study to find that trait anxiety moderates the association between state anxiety and amygdala-hippocampal activation to fear relative to happy facial affect. These results indicate that, among youth with moderate and high trait anxiety, state anxiety can exacerbate amygdala-hippocampal response to threat stimuli. Thus, fear or discomfort in MRI scanners can mimic or augment the effects of trait anxiety on amygdala-hippocampal activity, even among youth with mean levels of trait anxiety. During threat processing, the amygdala can also modulate brain activity in other brain regions, including perceptual [112, 113] and executive control networks [114,115,116]. Thus, amygdala hyperactivation associated with state anxiety may have widespread effects on brain function. Such compounding effects of trait and state anxiety have implications for investigating processes associated with clinical anxiety. For instance, moderately trait anxious youth categorized as “healthy controls” may exhibit elevated anticipatory state anxiety, causing them to resemble youth with high trait anxiety or clinical anxiety, thereby minimizing or obscuring differences between groups.

Surprisingly, for children with low trait anxiety, state anxiety was inversely associated with right amygdala-hippocampal activity for fearful compared to happy facial affect. In other words, low trait-anxious children with high state anxiety displayed greater right amygdala-hippocampal activity to happy faces, compared to fearful faces. This pattern differs from prior research finding state-anxious participants show greater amygdala response to fearful expressions [7, 117] and weaker amygdala reactivity to happy expressions [8]. Although our speculation is limited, it’s worth highlighting several potentially significant factors that could contribute to this result. Although better known for its role in threat processing, the amygdala also plays a role in reward processing and positive affect [50, 118], and therefore its activity may be elicited by happy faces. Certain personality traits are associated with individual differences in amygdala response to happy faces. For instance, extraversion, which is often higher in those with low trait anxiety [119], is associated with greater amygdala response to happy facial affect [120]. Thus, youth with low trait anxiety may display stronger amygdala-hippocampal responses to happy affect than their moderately and highly trait-anxious peers. However, in our sample, this pattern of activation was only observed in participants with both low trait anxiety and high state anxiety. Preliminary work finds that state anxiety can be associated with weaker amygdala response to fearful stimuli in specific contexts, such as following positive movies [121], suggesting that state-anxious participants might be more susceptible to the effects of positive emotional stimuli. Our data may therefore support a model whereby state anxiety can modulate participants’ susceptibility to both positive and negative biases; further testing would help confirm such a possibility.

Finally, we observed a three-way interaction between state anxiety, trait anxiety, and emotional valence in the left IPL. Again, for girls with mean and high levels of trait anxiety, state anxiety was positively associated with left IPL activity for fearful compared to happy facial affect. The IPL is involved in deliberate and sustained attention [122, 123], but elevated IPL activity during threat processing may also indicate hypervigilance [124]. Children with anxiety display hypervigilance to threats [1, 3, 15, 125,126,127,128,129] and right IPL hyperactivity during emotion processing [130]. Additionally, among anxious and non-anxious youth, negative affect is associated with left IPL-amygdala functional connectivity when appraising threat [131]. The pattern of activation in the IPL may therefore be related to parallel findings observed in the amygdala-hippocampal complex, though their contralateral location complicates this interpretation. Together, our results suggest that anticipatory anxiety in the scanning environment may cause children with moderate and high trait anxiety to be hypervigilant to threat, further compounding the effects of trait anxiety. These data align with prior work in adults showing that state anxiety is associated with hypervigilance to threat only for trait-anxious [70] and clinically anxious participants [132].

Several limitations of the current study and future research considerations should be acknowledged. First, our insula hypotheses were not confirmed, possibly because state and trait anxiety show overlapping patterns of neural activation in the insula [53], and thus, may not elicit interactive effects. Second, sample size was modest. Recent studies suggest that brain-behavior effects can sometimes be inflated and contribute to problems with replicability [133]. Further, examining interactive effects in modest samples may have limited our ability to detect significant effects, as a smaller proportion of the sample fell 1 SD above or below the mean on the STAIC-T. However, unlike some other methods to explore interactive effects, simple slopes analyses use the whole dataset to predict slopes at each level of the moderator, so these estimations were informed by the full sample. Despite the modest sample size, our study was strengthened by the fact that our sample consisted of Latina girls -- a demographic group that was under-represented in research. As mentioned previously, low representation of Latinx children in anxiety research is especially troubling given that they display high rates of anxiety [91,92,93,94, 97, 98]. Further, Latinx children are one of the largest and fastest-growing ethnic groups in the United States [134]. Thus, results may inform future large-scale studies by identifying preliminary effects within a well-characterized sample of Latina girls. Third, the cross-sectional study design limited our ability to make inferences about developmental processes. Future work should test longitudinal changes in the effects of state and trait anxiety on implicit fear processing, especially within executive control networks, which may display changing associations with anxiety across development. Fourth, because state anxiety measures were collected prior to the scan, we cannot be sure that such levels were sustained throughout the task. Thus, our results capture the effects of anticipatory, pre-scan anxiety on neural response during implicit emotion processing. A final limitation of this study is that all the faces presented in this experiment were non-Hispanic white people. All girls in this study identified as Latina and most were 100% Latina (~ 85%, N = 39), with the remainder of girls from both white and Latinx backgrounds (~ 15%, N = 7). Thus, for most participants, stimuli were from an outgroup (other) race-ethnicity. People respond differently to face stimuli that depict members of their own race compared with those of an outgroup race or ethnicity [135]. Outgroup members are more readily associated with aversive stimuli [136] and anxious arousal [137] than members of one’s own race or ethnicity. People also display differences in neural activation to racial ingroup versus outgroup faces [138, 139]. Thus, differences in participants’ experiences or familiarity with white people may have influenced their neural response to the face stimuli. Future work should sample faces from a variety of races and ethnicities and/or covary for participants’ experiences with racial outgroup members.

In summary, the present study examined the influences of trait anxiety and anticipatory, pre-scan state anxiety on Latina girls’ neural response to fearful and happy facial affect. Among girls with moderate and high levels of trait anxiety, state anxiety was associated with greater right amygdala-hippocampal and left IPL activity to fearful relative to happy facial affect. Together, these results suggest that anticipatory state anxiety in the scanning environment may cause children with moderate and high trait anxiety to be hypervigilant to threat, further compounding the effects of trait anxiety. Minoritized groups often have reduced engagement in scientific research and more mistrust [12,13,14], and thus may experience greater levels of pre-scan state anxiety. In the present study, girls who rated their family as having a lower community standing tended to have elevated pre-scan state anxiety (prior to multiple comparison correction), which may support that demographic factors like subjective social status influence children’s reaction to the research environment. Imaging researchers should survey and control for state anxiety so that any systematic differences in subgroups’ neural response resulting from MRI apprehension are not incorrectly attributed to demographic or environmental characteristics.

Data Availability

The data in the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

References

  1. Brotman MA, Rich BA, Schmajuk M, Reising M, Monk CS, Dickstein DP, et al. Attention bias to threat faces in children with bipolar disorder and comorbid lifetime anxiety disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:819–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Reeb-Sutherland BC, Rankin Williams L, Degnan KA, Pérez-Edgar K, Chronis-Tuscano A, Leibenluft E, et al. Identification of emotional facial expressions among behaviorally inhibited adolescents with lifetime anxiety disorders. Cogn Emot. 2015;29:372–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Roy AK, Vasa RA, Bruck M, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Sweeney M, et al. Attention bias toward threat in pediatric anxiety disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47:1189–96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Shechner T, Jarcho JM, Britton JC, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Nelson EE. Attention bias of anxious youth during extended exposure of emotional face pairs: an eye-tracking study. Depress Anxiety. 2013;30:14–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Blackford JU, Pine DS. Neural substrates of childhood anxiety disorders: a review of neuroimaging findings. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2012;21:501–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Stirling LJ, Eley TC, Clark DM. Preliminary evidence for an association between social anxiety symptoms and avoidance of negative faces in school-age children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2006;35:431–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Bishop SJ, Duncan J, Lawrence AD. State anxiety modulation of the amygdala response to unattended threat-related stimuli. J Neurosci. 2004;24:10364–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Somerville LH, Kim H, Johnstone T, Alexander AL, Whalen PJ. Human amygdala responses during presentation of happy and Neutral faces: correlations with state anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:897–903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Suzuki Y, Tanaka SC. Functions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion regulation under stress. Sci Rep. 2021;11:18225.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Skouras S, Gray M, Critchley H, Koelsch S. FMRI scanner noise interaction with affective neural processes. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e80564.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Meléndez JC, McCrank E. Anxiety-related reactions associated with magnetic resonance imaging examinations. JAMA. 1993;270:745–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Yancey AK, Ortega AN, Kumanyika SK. Effective recruitment and retention of minority research participants. Annu Rev Public Health. 2006;27:1–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Loue S, Sajatovic M. Research with severely mentally ill latinas: successful recruitment and retention strategies. J Immigr Minor Health. 2008;10:145–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Preloran HM, Browner CH, Lieber E. Strategies for motivating latino couples’ participation in qualitative health research and their effects on sample construction. Am J Public Health. 2001;91:1832–41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Telzer EH, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Mai X, Ernst M, Pine DS, et al. Relationship between trait anxiety, prefrontal cortex, and attention bias to angry faces in children and adolescents. Biol Psychol. 2008;79:216–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Abend R, Bajaj MA, Matsumoto C, Yetter M, Harrewijn A, Cardinale EM, et al. Converging multi-modal evidence for implicit threat-related Bias in Pediatric anxiety disorders. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. 2021;49:227–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Taghavi MR, Moradi AR, Neshat-Doost HT, Yule W, Dalgleish T. Interpretation of ambiguous emotional information in clinically anxious children and adolescents. Cogn Emot. 2000;14:809–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Muris P, Merckelbach H, Damsma E. Threat perception bias in nonreferred, socially anxious children. J Clin Child Psychol. 2000;29:348–59.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Maoz K, Eldar S, Stoddard J, Pine DS, Leibenluft E, Bar-Haim Y. Angry-happy interpretations of ambiguous faces in social anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2016;241:122–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Thomas KM, Drevets WC, Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Birmaher B, Eccard CH, et al. Amygdala response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed children. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:1057–63.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Killgore WDS, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Social anxiety predicts amygdala activation in adolescents viewing fearful faces. NeuroReport. 2005;16:1671–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Etkin A, Klemenhagen KC, Dudman JT, Rogan MT, Hen R, Kandel ER, et al. Individual differences in trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful faces. Neuron. 2004;44:1043–55.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Dickie EW, Armony JL. Amygdala responses to unattended fearful faces: Interaction between sex and trait anxiety. Psychiatry Res. 2008;162:51–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Günther V, Hußlack A, Weil A-S, Bujanow A, Henkelmann J, Kersting A et al. Individual differences in anxiety and automatic amygdala response to fearful faces: A replication and extension of Etkin (2004). Neuroimage Clin. 2020;28: 102441.

  25. Brühl AB, Delsignore A, Komossa K, Weidt S. Neuroimaging in social anxiety disorder—a meta-analytic review resulting in a new neurofunctional model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47:260–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Etkin A, Wager TD. Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:1476–88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Buff C, Brinkmann L, Neumeister P, Feldker K, Heitmann C, Gathmann B, et al. Specifically altered brain responses to threat in generalized anxiety disorder relative to social anxiety disorder and panic disorder. Neuroimage Clin. 2016;12:698–706.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Fonzo GA, Ramsawh HJ, Flagan TM, Sullivan SG, Letamendi A, Simmons AN, et al. Common and disorder-specific neural responses to emotional faces in generalised anxiety, social anxiety and panic disorders. Br J Psychiatry. 2015;206:206–15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Monk CS, Telzer EH, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Mai X, Louro HMC, et al. Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked angry faces in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:568–76.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Strawn JR, Bitter SM, Weber WA, Chu W-J, Whitsel RM, Adler C, et al. Neurocircuitry of generalized anxiety disorder in adolescents: a pilot functional neuroimaging and functional connectivity study. Depress Anxiety. 2012;29:939–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Monk CS, Nelson EE, McClure EB, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Leibenluft E, et al. Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation and attentional bias in response to angry faces in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1091–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, Parrish JM, Adler A, Blair RJR, et al. Abnormal attention modulation of fear circuit function in pediatric generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:97–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Swartz JR, Phan KL, Angstadt M, Klumpp H, Fitzgerald KD, Monk CS. Altered activation of the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in the context of emotional face distractors in children and adolescents with anxiety disorders. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31:870–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Glenn DE, Fox NA, Pine DS, Peters MAK, Michalska KJ. Divergence in cortical representations of threat generalization in affective versus perceptual circuitry in childhood: relations with anxiety. Neuropsychologia. 2020;142:107416.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Glenn DE, Merenstein JL, Bennett IJ, Michalska KJ. Anxiety symptoms and puberty interactively predict lower cingulum microstructure in preadolescent Latina girls. Sci Rep. 2022;12:20755.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Phan KL, Orlichenko A, Boyd E, Angstadt M, Coccaro EF, Liberzon I, et al. Preliminary evidence of white matter abnormality in the uncinate fasciculus in generalized social anxiety disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:691–4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Baur V, Hänggi J, Jäncke L. Volumetric associations between uncinate fasciculus, amygdala, and trait anxiety. BMC Neurosci. 2012;13:4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Eden AS, Schreiber J, Anwander A, Keuper K, Laeger I, Zwanzger P, et al. Emotion regulation and trait anxiety are predicted by the microstructure of fibers between amygdala and prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2015;35:6020–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Guyer AE, Lau JYF, McClure-Tone EB, Parrish J, Shiffrin ND, Reynolds RC, et al. Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex function during anticipated peer evaluation in pediatric social anxiety. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:1303–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Pagliaccio D, Luby JL, Bogdan R, Agrawal A, Gaffrey MS, Belden AC, et al. Amygdala functional connectivity, HPA axis genetic variation, and life stress in children and relations to anxiety and emotion regulation. J Abnorm Psychol. 2015;124:817–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Roy AK, Fudge JL, Kelly C, Perry JSA, Daniele T, Carlisi C, et al. Intrinsic functional connectivity of amygdala-based networks in adolescent generalized anxiety disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52:290–299e2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Etkin A. Functional neuroanatomy of anxiety: a neural circuit perspective. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2010;2:251–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Etkin A. Neurobiology of anxiety: from neural circuits to novel solutions? Depress Anxiety. 2012;29:355–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Knight DC, Nguyen HT, Bandettini PA. The role of the human amygdala in the production of conditioned fear responses. NeuroImage. 2005;26:1193–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Davis M, Whalen PJ. The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry. 2001;6:13–34.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Tayah T, Savard M, Desbiens R, Nguyen DK. Ictal bradycardia and asystole in an adult with a focal left insular lesion. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2013;115:1885–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Uddin LQ, Nomi JS, Hébert-Seropian B, Ghaziri J, Boucher O. Structure and function of the human insula. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2017;34:300–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R. Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:504–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Murray EA. The amygdala, reward and emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:489–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Pine DS, Wise SP, Murray EA. Evolution, emotion, and Episodic Engagement. Am J Psychiatry. 2021;178:701–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Tyc VL, Fairclough D, Fletcher B, Leigh L, Mulhern RK. Children’s distress during magnetic resonance imaging procedures. Child Health Care. 1995;24:5–19.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Saviola F, Pappaianni E, Monti A, Grecucci A, Jovicich J, De Pisapia N. Trait and state anxiety are mapped differently in the human brain. Sci Rep. 2020;10:11112.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Chavanne AV, Robinson OJ. The overlapping neurobiology of Induced and pathological anxiety: a Meta-analysis of functional neural activation. Am J Psychiatry. 2021;178:156–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Michalska KJ, Gardiner G, Hughes BL. 28 what Neuroscience can tell us about social situations. The Oxford Handbook of Psychological Situations; 2020. p. 437.

  55. Raz A, Lieber B, Soliman F, Buhle J, Posner J, Peterson BS, et al. Ecological nuances in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): psychological stressors, posture, and hydrostatics. NeuroImage. 2005;25:1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Chou I-J, Tench CR, Gowland P, Jaspan T, Dineen RA, Evangelou N, et al. Subjective discomfort in children receiving 3 T MRI and experienced adults’ perspective on children’s tolerability of 7 T: a cross-sectional questionnaire survey. BMJ Open. 2014;e006094. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmjopen-2014-006094.

  57. Eatough EM, Shirtcliff EA, Hanson JL, Pollak SD. Hormonal reactivity to MRI scanning in adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:1242–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Lueken U, Muehlhan M, Evens R, Wittchen H-U, Kirschbaum C. Within and between session changes in subjective and neuroendocrine stress parameters during magnetic resonance imaging: a controlled scanner training study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1299–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Attwood AS, Easey KE, Dalili MN, Skinner AL, Woods A, Crick L, et al. State anxiety and emotional face recognition in healthy volunteers. R Soc Open Sci. 2017;4:160855.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Dyer ML, Attwood AS, Penton-Voak IS, Munafò MR. The role of state and trait anxiety in the processing of facial expressions of emotion. R Soc Open Sci. 2022;9:210056.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Quigley L, Nelson AL, Carriere J, Smilek D, Purdon C. The effects of trait and state anxiety on attention to emotional images: an eye-tracking study. Cogn Emot. 2012;26:1390–411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Nelson AL, Purdon C, Quigley L, Carriere J, Smilek D. Distinguishing the roles of trait and state anxiety on the nature of anxiety-related attentional biases to threat using a free viewing eye movement paradigm. Cogn Emot. 2015;29:504–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Li W, Howard JD, Parrish TB, Gottfried JA. Aversive learning enhances perceptual and cortical discrimination of Indiscriminable odor cues. Science. 2008. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1126/science.1152837. [cited 9 Nov 2021].

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Cornwell BR, Garrido MI, Overstreet C, Pine DS, Grillon C. The unpredictive brain under threat: a neurocomputational account of anxious hypervigilance. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;82:447–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Karvay Y, Imbriano G, Jin J, Mohanty A, Jarcho JM. They’re watching you: the impact of social evaluation and anxiety on threat-related perceptual decision-making. Psychol Res. 2022;86:1174–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Kobald SO, Getzmann S, Beste C, Wascher E. The impact of simulated MRI scanner background noise on visual attention processes as measured by the EEG. Sci Rep. 2016;6:28371.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  67. Kavcıoğlu FC, Bublatzky F, Pittig A, Alpers GW. Instructed threat enhances threat perception in faces. Emotion. 2021;21:419–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Muris P, Rapee R, Meesters C, Schouten E, Geers M. Threat perception abnormalities in children: the role of anxiety disorders symptoms, chronic anxiety, and state anxiety. J Anxiety Disord. 2003;17:271–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Baur V, Hänggi J, Langer N, Jäncke L. Resting-state functional and structural connectivity within an insula-amygdala route specifically index state and trait anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73:85–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Egloff B, Hock M. Interactive effects of state anxiety and trait anxiety on emotional Stroop interference. Pers Individ Dif. 2001;31:875–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Rutherford E, MacLeod C, Campbell L. BRIEF REPORT negative selectivity effects and emotional selectivity effects in anxiety: Differential attentional correlates of state and trait variables. Cogn Emot. 2004;18:711–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Marshall SP, Smith MS, Weinberger E. Perceived anxiety of pediatric patients to magnetic resonance. Clin Pediatr. 1995;34:59–60.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. Chapman HA, Bernier D, Rusak B. MRI-related anxiety levels change within and between repeated scanning sessions. Psychiatry Res. 2010;182:160–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Rosenberg DR, Sweeney JA, Gillen JS, Kim J, Varanelli MJ, O’Hearn KM, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging of children without sedation: preparation with simulation. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:853–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  75. Webb EK, Etter JA, Kwasa JA. Addressing racial and phenotypic bias in human neuroscience methods. Nat Neurosci. 2022;25:410–4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Stein MB, Simmons AN, Feinstein JS, Paulus MP. Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:318–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Lau JYF, Guyer AE, Tone EB, Jenness J, Parrish JM, Pine DS, et al. Neural responses to peer rejection in anxious adolescents: contributions from the amygdala-hippocampal complex. Int J Behav Dev. 2012;36:36–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Spielberg JM, Jarcho JM, Dahl RE, Pine DS, Ernst M, Nelson EE. Anticipation of peer evaluation in anxious adolescents: divergence in neural activation and maturation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10:1084–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav Brain Sci. 2010;33:61–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychol. 2000;19:586–92.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  81. Spielberger CD, Edwards CD, Montouri J, Lushene R. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children. 1972. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/t06497-000.

  82. Spielberger CD, Edwards CD, Lushene RE, Montuori J, Platzek D. State-trait anxiety inventory for children: preliminary manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists; 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Birmaher B, Khetarpal S, Brent D, Cully M, Balach L, Kaufman J, et al. The screen for child anxiety related Emotional disorders (SCARED): scale construction and psychometric characteristics. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:545–53.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  84. Behrens B, Swetlitz C, Pine DS, Pagliaccio D. The screen for child anxiety related Emotional disorders (SCARED): informant discrepancy, Measurement Invariance, and test-retest reliability. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2019;50:473–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Ivarsson T, Skarphedinsson G, Andersson M, Jarbin H. The validity of the screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders revised (SCARED-R) scale and Sub-scales in Swedish Youth. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2018;49:234–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Blair RJR, Colledge E, Murray L, Mitchell DG. A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2001;29:491–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  87. Stoddard J, Tseng W-L, Kim P, Chen G, Yi J, Donahue L, et al. Association of Irritability and anxiety with the neural mechanisms of Implicit face emotion Processing in youths with psychopathology. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74:95–103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res. 1996;29(3):162–73.

  89. Chen G, Saad ZS, Britton JC, Pine DS, Cox RW. Linear mixed-effects modeling approach to FMRI group analysis. NeuroImage. 2013;73:176–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. La Scala S, Mullins JL, Firat RB, Emotional Learning Research Community Advisory Board, Michalska KJ. Equity, diversity, and inclusion in developmental neuroscience: practical lessons from community-based participatory research. Front Integr Neurosci. 2022;16:1007249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Varela RE, Sanchez-Sosa JJ, Biggs BK, Luis TM. Anxiety symptoms and fears in hispanic and European American Children: cross-cultural measurement equivalence. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2008;30:132–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Potochnick SR, Perreira KM. Depression and anxiety among first-generation immigrant latino youth: key correlates and implications for future research. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2010;198:470–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  93. Polo AJ, Solano-Martinez JE, Saldana L, Ramos AD, Herrera M, Ullrich T et al. The epidemic of internalizing problems among Latinx adolescents before and during the Coronavirus 2019 Pandemic. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2023; 1–17.

  94. McLaughlin KA, Hilt LM, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Racial/ethnic differences in internalizing and externalizing symptoms in adolescents. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2007;35:801–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Bennett M, Roche KM, Huebner DM, Lambert SF. School discrimination and changes in Latinx adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing symptoms. J Youth Adolesc. 2020;49:2020–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Schlaudt VA, Suarez-Morales L, Black RA. Exploring the relationship of Acculturative stress and anxiety symptoms in latino youth. Child Youth Care Forum. 2021;50:261–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Varela RE, Niditch LA, Hensley-Maloney L, Moore KW, Creveling CC, Jones KM. Culture specific influences on anxiety in latino youth. Child Youth Care Forum. 2019;48:1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Varela RE, Sanchez-Sosa JJ, Biggs BK, Luis TM. Parenting strategies and socio-cultural influences in childhood anxiety: Mexican, latin American descent, and European American families. J Anxiety Disord. 2009;23:609–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Charpentier CJ, Faulkner P, Pool ER, Ly V, Tollenaar MS, Kluen LM, et al. How representative are neuroimaging samples? Large-scale evidence for trait anxiety differences between fMRI and behaviour-only research participants. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2021;16:1057–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. Endler NS, Kocovski NL. State and trait anxiety revisited. J Anxiety Disord. 2001;15:231–45.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  101. Angel Soto J, Roberts NA, Pole N, Levenson RW, Burleson MH, King AR, et al. Elevated baseline anxiety among African americans in Laboratory Research Settings. J Psychophysiol. 2012;26:105–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Phelps EA. Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:198–202.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  103. Richardson MP, Strange BA, Dolan RJ. Encoding of emotional memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus and their interactions. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:278–85.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  104. Richter-Levin G. The amygdala, the hippocampus, and emotional modulation of memory. Neuroscientist. 2004;10:31–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Carlson JM, Reinke KS, Habib R. A left amygdala mediated network for rapid orienting to masked fearful faces. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1386–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Monk CS, Nelson EE, Woldehawariat G, Montgomery LA, Zarahn E, McClure EB, et al. Experience-dependent plasticity for attention to threat: behavioral and neurophysiological evidence in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:607–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Anderson AK, Phelps EA. Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature. 2001;411:305–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  108. Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S. The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science. 1991;253:1380–6.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  109. Satpute AB, Mumford JA, Naliboff BD, Poldrack RA. Human anterior and posterior hippocampus respond distinctly to state and trait anxiety. Emotion. 2012;12:58–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Milad MR, Wright CI, Orr SP, Pitman RK, Quirk GJ, Rauch SL. Recall of fear extinction in humans activates the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in concert. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:446–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Lau JYF, Goldman D, Buzas B, Hodgkinson C, Leibenluft E, Nelson E, et al. BDNF gene polymorphism (Val66Met) predicts amygdala and anterior hippocampus responses to emotional faces in anxious and depressed adolescents. NeuroImage. 2010;53:952–61.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  112. Pourtois G, Schettino A, Vuilleumier P. Brain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention: what is magic and what is not. Biol Psychol. 2013;92:492–512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Sussman TJ, Jin J, Mohanty A. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in anxiety. Biol Psychol. 2016;121:160–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Garcia R, Vouimba RM, Baudry M, Thompson RF. The amygdala modulates prefrontal cortex activity relative to conditioned fear. Nature. 1999;402:294–6.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  115. Klavir O, Prigge M, Sarel A, Paz R, Yizhar O. Manipulating fear associations via optogenetic modulation of amygdala inputs to prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20:836–44.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  116. Likhtik E, Paz R. Amygdala-prefrontal interactions in (mal)adaptive learning. Trends Neurosci. 2015;38:158–66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  117. Cornwell BR, Alvarez RP, Lissek S, Kaplan R, Ernst M, Grillon C. Anxiety overrides the blocking effects of high perceptual load on amygdala reactivity to threat-related distractors. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1363–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Baxter MG, Murray EA. The amygdala and reward. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:563–73.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  119. Jylhä P, Isometsä E. The relationship of neuroticism and extraversion to symptoms of anxiety and depression in the general population. Depress Anxiety. 2006;23:281–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Canli T, Sivers H, Whitfield SL, Gotlib IH, Gabrieli JDE. Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science. 2002;296:2191.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  121. Pichon S, Miendlarzewska EA, Eryilmaz H, Vuilleumier P. Cumulative activation during positive and negative events and state anxiety predicts subsequent inertia of amygdala reactivity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10:180–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  122. Behrmann M, Geng JJ, Shomstein S. Parietal cortex and attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:212–7.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  123. Singh-Curry V, Husain M. The functional role of the inferior parietal lobe in the dorsal and ventral stream dichotomy. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1434–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  124. Balderston NL, Hale E, Hsiung A, Torrisi S, Holroyd T, Carver FW, et al. Threat of shock increases excitability and connectivity of the intraparietal sulcus. Elife. 2017;6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.7554/eLife.23608.

  125. Cisler JM, Koster EHW. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30:203–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Shechner T, Britton JC, Pérez-Edgar K, Bar-Haim Y, Ernst M, Fox NA, et al. Attention biases, anxiety, and development: toward or away from threats or rewards? Depress Anxiety. 2012;29:282–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. Fox E, Russo R, Dutton K. Attentional Bias for threat: evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cogn Emot. 2002;16:355–79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  128. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull. 2007;133:1–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  129. Michalska KJ, Benson B, Ivie EJ, Sachs JF, Haller SP, Abend R, et al. Neural responding during uncertain threat anticipation in pediatric anxiety. Int J Psychophysiol. 2023;183:159–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  130. Díaz DE, Russman Block S, Becker HC, Luan Phan K, Monk CS, Fitzgerald KD. Age and attentional focus alter the neural substrates of anxiety severity in youth during emotion processing. Under review.

  131. Tseng W-L, Abend R, Gold AL, Brotman MA. Neural correlates of extinguished threat recall underlying the commonality between pediatric anxiety and irritability. J Affect Disord. 2021;295:920–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  132. Wermes R, Lincoln TM, Helbig-Lang S. Anxious and alert? Hypervigilance in social anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2018;269:740–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  133. Marek S, Tervo-Clemmens B, Calabro FJ, Montez DF, Kay BP, Hatoum AS, et al. Publisher correction: reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature. 2022;605:E11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  134. Colby SL, Ortman JM. Projections of the Size and Composition of the US Population: 2014 to 2060. Population Estimates and Projections. Current Population Reports. P25-1143. US Census Bureau. 2015. Available: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED578934.

  135. Fiske ST. Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, editors. Handbook of social psychology. Boston: McGraw-Hill; 1998. pp. 1–2.

    Google Scholar 

  136. Navarrete CD, McDonald MM, Asher BD, Kerr NL, Yokota K, Olsson A, et al. Fear is readily associated with an out-group face in a minimal group context. Evol Hum Behav. 2012;33:590–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  137. Olsson A, Ebert JP, Banaji MR, Phelps EA. The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science. 2005;309:785–7.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  138. Hart AJ, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Fischer H, Rauch SL. Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. NeuroReport. 2000;11:2351–5.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  139. Golby AJ, Gabrieli JD, Chiao JY, Eberhardt JL. Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:845–50.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  140. Cooney RE, Atlas LY, Joormann J, Eugène F, Gotlib IH. Amygdala activation in the processing of Neutral faces in social anxiety disorder: is Neutral really Neutral? Psychiatry Res. 2006;148:55–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  141. Winton EC, Clark DM, Edelmann RJ. Social anxiety, fear of negative evaluation and the detection of negative emotion in others. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33:193–6.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the children and families who participated in the study. We also thank the research staff who contributed to this study, including Sarah Carlisle.

Funding

Support for this study was provided by a grant from the Hellman Fellows Program and a National Institute of Health subaward (U54MD013368) from the UCR Center for Health Disparities Research to Dr. Kalina J. Michalska.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

K.J.M. and D.E.D. conceptualized study design. D.E.D. collected the data. K.J.M and W.L.T. oversaw data analysis. D.E.D. performed data analysis and wrote the manuscript. All authors reviewed, edited, and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dana E. Díaz.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Study procedures were approved by the University of California, Riverside Institutional Review Board (Study HS-17-208) and all experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent and assent were obtained at the start of the first visit from parents and children, respectively.

Consent for publication

Written informed consent and assent were obtained at the start of the first visit from parents and children, respectively.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Díaz, D.E., Tseng, WL. & Michalska, K.J. Pre-scan state anxiety is associated with greater right amygdala-hippocampal response to fearful versus happy faces among trait-anxious Latina girls. BMC Psychiatry 24, 1 (2024). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12888-023-05403-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12888-023-05403-6

Keywords