Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Theory of mind, emotion recognition, delusions and the quality of the therapeutic relationship in patients with psychosis – a secondary analysis of a randomized-controlled therapy trial

Abstract

Background

Cognitive models of psychosis postulate an important role of Theory of mind (ToM) in the formation and maintenance of delusions, but research on this plausible conjecture has gathered conflicting findings. In addition, it is still an open question whether problems in emotion recognition (ER) are associated with delusions. We examined the association of problems in ToM and ER with different aspects of delusions in a large sample of patients with psychosis enrolled in a therapy trial. This also enabled us to explore the possible impact of ToM and ER on one part of patients’ social life: the quality of their therapeutic relationship.

Methods

Patients with psychotic disorders and delusions and/or hallucinations (n = 185) and healthy controls (n = 48) completed a ToM picture sequencing task and an ER task. Subsequently, patients were enrolled in a randomized-controlled Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT) trial (ISRCTN29242879). Patients and therapists rated the quality of the therapeutic relationship during the first five sessions of therapy.

Results

In comparison to controls, patients were impaired in both ToM and ER. Patients with deficits in ER experienced more severe delusional distress, whereas ToM problems were not related to delusions. In addition, deficits in ER predicted a less favorable therapeutic relationship and interactional problems viewed by the therapist. Impaired ER also moderated (increased) the negative influence of delusions on the therapeutic relationship and interactional difficulties viewed by the therapist.

Conclusions

Cognitive models on the formation and maintenance of delusions should consider ER as a potential candidate that might be related to the formation and maintenance of delusional distress, whereas problems in ToM might not be directly related to delusions and secondary dimensions of delusions. In addition, problems in ER in patients with psychosis might have an impact on the quality of the therapeutic relationship and patients with problems in ER are more likely to be viewed as problematic by their therapists. Nevertheless, training ER might be a way to improve the quality of the therapeutic relationship and potentially the effectiveness of CBT or other interventions for patients with psychosis.

Peer Review reports

Background

Theory of Mind (ToM) is defined as the ability to attribute other peoples’ mental states, intentions and emotions and to understand and predict their behaviour [1]. It is part of the concept of social cognition, i.e. the perception, processing and interpretation of social signals [2, 3]. Frith [4] proposed that deficits in ToM are a predisposing factor for persecutory delusions. Based on this assumption, ToM in patients with psychosis was found to be more than one standard deviation below the performance of healthy controls [5, 6]. In addition, several studies found evidence of an association between ToM deficits and persecutory [7] and general delusions [8]. Consequently, ToM has been incorporated into theoretical models as one causal factor involved in the formation and maintenance of delusions [9, 10] and positive symptoms of psychosis [11].

Nevertheless, in a recent review, ToM problems were associated with more severe general delusions or delusions of persecution in about half of all studies that addressed this question, whereas the other studies did not find an association [12]. Consequently, several newer theoretical models excluded ToM problems as a causal factor for persecutory delusions or positive symptoms [12, 13].

One explanation for the inconsistent results could be the small sample size of most studies investigating associations between ToM and delusions that was also not mended by the latest meta-analysis: rather than assessing the relation between ToM and delusions the authors investigated the relationship of ToM and reality distortion (an aggregation of delusions and hallucinations) and did not find any evidence for an association [14].

In addition, it is possible that problems in ToM are rather a risk factor for psychosis in general than delusions, whereas more basal problems of patients with psychosis in emotion recognition (ER), the ability to identify other persons’ emotions by using emotionally salient information in the environment (verbal and non-verbal cues) [15] might be more important in the formation and maintenance of delusions. This is suggested by the fact that patients with psychosis are severely impaired in ER in comparison to controls [16, 17] and by the results of the meta-analysis mentioned above that suggest an association between problems in ER and reality distortion [14, 18].

Further, as other social-cognitive biases as the jumping to conclusions-bias are related with more severe delusional frequency and delusional distress [19], it is possible that problems in ToM and ER problems might also enhance the frequency and distress caused by delusions, this question has not been addressed until today.

While an associations with delusions is unclear, ToM problems in patients with psychosis are closely related to problems with their social performance such as inadequate eye contact, speech modulation and conversational flow [20], interpersonal difficulties [21, 22] and problems in social functioning [23, 24]. Nevertheless, it is unclear whether problems in ToM and ER in patients with psychosis also affect the quality of their social interactions in real life.

One paradigm of a social interaction in real life is the therapeutic alliance defined as affective bond and consensus with regard to goals and treatment tasks [25]. As the therapeutic alliance is highly important for the effectiveness of Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT [26,27,28]), it is crucial to assess whether problems in ToM and ER might have a negative effect on it. This question has been investigated in one study reporting associations between ToM problems and patients’ ratings on the quality of the therapeutic alliance [29]. With regard to problems in ER, their impact on the therapeutic alliance has not been addressed until today, but an impact might be possible. If either ToM problems and/or deficits in ER might be associated with the therapeutic relationship, Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT) should more strongly address patients’ problems in ToM and ER to improve the therapeutic alliance and consequently its effectiveness.

Besides TOM and ER abilities, persecutory delusions also negatively impact the therapeutic alliance in CBT [30]. Interestingly, preserved ToM abilities were found to moderate (reduce) the negative influence of persecutory delusions on social functioning [31]. Thus, it is likely that positive ToM and ER performance might protect patients from the additional negative impact of delusions on the quality of the therapeutic relationship. Patients with preserved ToM and ER performance might be more able to understand the intentions and emotions of their therapists correctly and act accordingly, while still assuming that other persons are to be mistrusted. But, the question whether intact ToM and ER performance might reduce (moderate) the negative impact of delusions on the therapeutic relationship has not been assessed until today.

The present study was a secondary analysis of a randomised-controlled therapy trial [32] and was set out to investigate whether problems in ToM and ER are more severe in patients with psychosis (in comparison to controls (hypothesis 1)) and whether they are related to delusions and secondary dimensions of delusions (frequency and distress) in a large patient sample (hypothesis 2). Both hypothesis 1 and 2 have been pre-specified in the trial proposal. In an additional exploratory analysis, the study aimed to investigate, whether problems in ToM and ER influence the quality of the therapeutic relationship (hypothesis 3). Further, we assessed in an additional exploratory analysis, whether positive ToM and ER performance moderate the association between delusions and the quality of the therapeutic relationship (hypothesis 4).

Methods

Subjects

Subjects were 185 patients with psychosis and 48 healthy controls from the “Cognitive behavioural therapy for persistent positive symptoms (CBTp) in psychotic disorders” Trial [32] (ISRCTN29242879), a multi-centred randomized controlled trial investigating the efficacy of CBT for patients with psychosis in comparison to supportive therapy.

Inclusion criteria were a diagnosis of a psychotic disorder (schizophrenia (n = 147), schizophreniform disorder (n = 1), schizoaffective disorder (n = 25) or delusional disorder (n = 12)) assessed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID [33]).

Further inclusion criteria were persistent positive symptoms for at least the last three months and a minimum score of four in the item P1 (delusions in general: n = 162) and/or in the item P3 (hallucinations: n = 79; both: n = 56) of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS [34]), age between 18 and 59, adequate language fluency and a verbal intelligence quotient > 80 in the German IQ test Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatztest (MWT-B [35]. Exclusion criteria for healthy controls were mental disorders in their lifetime as assessed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID [33]).

Patients were recruited from six different psychiatric centers (Bonn, Cologne, Duesseldorf, Duisburg, Frankfurt am Main, Tuebingen, Germany); healthy controls were recruited in all six centers via public advertisement and matched with regard to age, gender, and education to the first 48 patients that were already recruited.

From the initial study sample (n = 330), a small number of patients dropped out before they were asked to participate (n = 9) or refused to participate in the additional assessment (n = 22), had problems with their eyesight (n = 3), with German language (n = 4), with test instructions (n = 14) or decided to participate in a nested fMRI study instead (n = 93). There were no statistically significant differences between the patients (n = 185) who performed in ToM and ER in the present study and patients who participated in the fMRI paradigm and between patients who refused to be tested and patients who endorsed testing with regard to sociodemographic and clinical variables (all p > .10).

All participants were informed about the assessment and gave written informed consent. In case of a legal guardian, the patients and the guardian were informed about the assessment and both the patient and the guardian gave written informed consent. The ethics committees at the six centres’ medical faculties approved the study. The randomized-controlled trial and the secondary analysis presented here adhere to Consort criteria.

From the total patient sample, a smaller number of patients participated in at least three therapy sessions and these patients were included in analyses regarding the association between ToM, ER, delusions and the therapeutic relationship (n = 174, CBT: n = 90, ST: n = 84).

Measures

Theory of Mind (ToM) performance was assessed using a cartoon task paradigm [36, 37] that presented excellent test-retest reliability with regard to activation of key ToM areas in previous studies [38] Participants were asked to view 14 comic strips presented on a computer screen in pseudo-randomized order. Each comic strip included two phases: in Phase I, three pictures (3 s each) depicting an unfolding story plot were shown sequentially. In Phase II, two answer pictures were presented simultaneously (26 s), and participants were asked to choose the picture showing the logical ending of the story. The comic strips presented a social interaction between two protagonists (e.g. a person asks a second person for a glass of water). In order to solve the task, participants were required to infer the intentions of the characters correctly. The sum scores of correct answers were used as measure for ToM (range between 0 and 14).

Emotion Recognition (ER) was assessed with a Pictures of Facial Affect test (PFA). 28 faces (10 photographs selected from Ekman and Friesen’s pictures of facial affect [39] and 18 photographs from a comparable set of pictures [40, 41]) served as stimuli depicting four basic emotions (fear, anger, disgust and sadness). Each emotion was displayed by seven different faces showing each emotion once. Faces were presented sequentially in randomized order and participants were asked to select the most applicable emotion from a multiple-choice list. The PFA sum score of correct answers was used as measure of ER (ER total score; range between 0 and 28). In addition, scores of the four negative emotions were also used (ER fear, ER anger, ER sadness and ER disgust; ranges between 0 and 7).

The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS [34]) is a semi-structured interview assessing 30 symptoms divided into three scales (positive scale, negative scale, general psychopathology scale, ranges between 7 and 14 for the positive and negative scale and between 16 and 30 for the general psychopathology scale) using a 7-point Likert scale. PANSS ratings were performed by trained raters who received ten training sessions in all items. The inter-rater reliability (ICC, corr. R2) was satisfactory to high (between .86 and .92). The item P1 (general delusions) was used as assessment of general delusions and the item P6 as assessments of delusions of persecution (range of both items between 1 and 7).

The Psychotic Symptom Rating Scale (PSYRATS [42]) is a semi-structured interview with six items assessing different dimensions of delusional beliefs such as amount of preoccupation, duration of preoccupation, conviction, disruption of daily life, amount of distress and intensity of distress on a 5-point-Likert scale (scores between 0 and 4). Inter-rater-reliability and validity were high in a sample of patients with psychosis [42]. Based on results of a factor analysis [43], the items are summed up to the subscale PSYRATS delusional frequency (sum of the subscales amount of preoccupation, duration of preoccupation, conviction and disruption of daily life, range between 0 and 16) and PSYRATS delusional distress (sum of the subscales amount of distress and intensity of distress, range between 0 and 8).

The quality of the therapeutic relationship was assessed using the short versions of the Patient Session Questionnaire (PSQ) and the Therapist Session Questionnaire (TSQ) derived from the German Berner Therapist- and Patient- Session Questionnaire [Berner Patienten- und Therapeutenstundenbogen 2000]. Internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) was found to be good and effect sizes of correlations between quality of the therapeutic relationship viewed by the patient and therapist were generally large [44]. Both therapists and patients answered the questionnaires after the first five sessions of either CBT or ST. In order to reduce socially more desirable answers, patients answered the questionnaire after every session and put their ratings in a closed box.

From the PSQ, the quality of the therapeutic alliance viewed by the patient subscale (PSQ therapy alliance) was used that consists of the sum of three items that were answered by patients after a therapeutic session on a 7-point-Likert scale (scores between − 3 and + 3) and reflect the quality of the therapeutic relationship viewed by the patient (“The therapist and I understand each other”, “Today I felt at ease with the therapist”, “I think the therapist is really interested in my well-being”). In addition, from the TSQ, the 3-item subscale TSQ therapist alliance was used that measures the mean score of the quality of the therapeutic relationship viewed by the therapist. Items are answered on a 6-point-Likert scale (“The patient and I understand each other.” “Today I felt at ease with the patient.” “The patient and I work on joint goals.”; scores between − 3 and + 3). The mean score over the first five sessions was used (range between − 3 and + 3).

In addition, we used one item in the TSQ covering interpersonal difficulties viewed by the therapist (TSQ interactional difficulties: “I believe this patient is difficult in terms of his/her interaction”) that was answered on a 7-point Likert scale (scores between − 3 and + 3). Again, mean scores of the first five sessions were used (range between − 3 and + 3).

Statistical analysis

First, we used Fisher’s exact tests, Chi2 tests and t-tests in order to compare patients with schizophrenia and controls in socio-demographic, clinical variables and ToM / ER problems (hypothesis 1). Exploration of the raw data showed ceiling effects for ToM and ER (total score and scores of the specific four negative emotions) as defined by Uttl [45] Thus, due to the skewedness of the data, we used an arcus-sinus-transformation as recommended by [46] that allows application of parametric statistics.

Hypothesis 2 (associations between ToM, ER and delusions) was assessed using Pearson’s bivariate correlations between ToM, ER (ER total score, ER fear, ER anger, ER sadness and ER disgust) and delusions in general (PANSS item P1), delusions of persecution (PANSS item P6) and the two PSYRATS subscales delusional frequency and delusional distress. Spearman rank correlations were used to examine hypothesis 3 (associations between ToM, ER total score, ER fear, ER anger, ER sadness and ER disgust and the therapeutic relationship), as PSQ and TSQ subscales were not normally distributed.

A series of hierarchical regression analyses were performed to assess whether ToM and ER problems moderate the association between delusions and the quality of the therapeutic relationship (hypothesis 4). All measures that were bivariately associated with the quality of the therapeutic relationship (PSQ therapy alliance / TSQ therapist alliance / TSQ interactional difficulties), were mean-centered and then included as predictors in the first block of the hierarchical regression analysis and their interaction term in the second block. General symptom severity (PANSS total score) was included as covariate.

Significant moderating relationships were analysed using the Johnson-Neyman technique included in the PROCESS macro [47], a follow-up method for regressions containing interaction coefficients that enables to identify over what range of the moderator a predictor has significant versus non-significant effects on the outcome measure [48].

Results

Table 1 shows socio-demographic and clinical data of patients with psychosis and healthy controls. There were no statistically significant differences between both groups in terms of age or gender. Compared to controls, patients showed significantly lower verbal intelligence scores (MWT-B), but associations between ToM, ER and verbal intelligence were small and not statistically significant and no statistical adjustment for verbal intelligence was performed.

Table 1 Sociodemographic and clinical variables of patients with psychosis and healthy controls

Group differences in ToM and ER (hypothesis 1: pre-specified analysis)

With regard to hypothesis 1, results revealed that patients with psychosis presented more pronounced deficits in both ToM and ER (descriptive scores are depicted in Fig. 1). With regard to ER, patients were more impaired than controls in the total ER score and in recognizing the emotions anger and disgust, whereas there were no statistically significant differences between both groups with regard to recognizing the emotions fear and sadness.

Fig. 1
figure 1

ToM and Emotion Recognition in patients with psychosis and healthy controls. Notes: ToM = Theory of Mind, ER = Emotion Recognition; Patients with psychosis: n = 185; Healthy controls: Theory of Mind: n = 42; Emotion Recognition: n = 39, error bars display the standard deviation

Associations between ToM, ER and delusions (hypothesis 2: pre-specified analysis)

As depicted in Table 2, there was no statistically significant association between ToM problems and any measure of delusions. However, problems in ER were related with more pronounced PSYRATS delusional distress. Specifically, delusional distress (PSYRATS delusional distress) showed a statistically significant association with the ability to recognize the emotions fear and disgust (ER fear and ER disgust), whereas the two other specific ER scores (ER anger and ER distrust) were not associated with delusional distress. Further, neither the ER total score nor the four specific negative emotions scores were related to any other measure of delusions.

Table 2 Results of Pearson correlation analyses between Theory of Mind, emotion recognition and delusions in the patient sample (n = 185)

Associations between ToM, ER, delusions and the quality of the therapeutic relationship (hypothesis 3: exploratory analysis)

Patients’ ratings of the quality of the therapeutic relationship (PSQ therapy alliance) were not related with ToM nor ER total score and ER subscales (see Table 3). In addition, there was no statistically significant association between PSQ therapy alliance and all other measures of delusions. Therapists’ ratings of the quality of the therapeutic relationship (TSQ therapy alliance) were related to ER total score and to all four ER subscales (ER fear, ER anger, ER sadness and ER disgust) and PSYRATS delusional frequency. TSQ interactional problems showed a statistically significant association with ER total score and PSYRATS delusional frequency. In addition, TSQ interactional problems were related with problems in recognizing the emotions fear and disgust (ER fear, ER disgust), whereas there were no statistically significant associations between TSQ interactional problems and problems in recognizing anger and sadness (ER anger and ER sadness).

Table 3 Results of Spearman correlation analyses between Theory of Mind, emotion recognition and the quality of the therapeutic relationship in the patient sample

Moderation analyses (hypothesis 4: exploratory analysis)

As can be seen in Table 4, including ER and PSYRATS delusional frequency in the first block of the hierarchical regression analysis predicting TSQ therapist alliance resulted in a statistically significant model, but solely ER was a statistically significant predictor of TSQ therapist alliance. Including the interaction term in the second block resulted in a statistically significant model and the interaction explained additional variance in TSQ therapy alliance, suggesting a statistically significant moderation effect.

Table 4 Hierarchical multiple regression analyses predicting the quality of the therapeutic alliance and interactional problems

The Johnson-Neyman technique revealed that ER was predictive of TSQ therapy alliance for participants with ER scores lower than 20.45% and above 79.65% (all p < 0.05). This suggests that in patients with scores in ER in percentiles between zero and 20.45, ER problems attenuated the negative influence of delusional frequency on TSQ therapy alliance and in patients with ER scores in percentiles above 79.65%, good ER ability protected them from the negative impact of delusional frequency on TSQ therapy alliance.

Results of the second hierarchical regression analysis revealed a statistically significant model including ER and PSYRATS delusional frequency as predictors of TSQ interactional problems, but solely ER independently predicted interactional problems, whereas PSYRATS delusional frequency was not independently predictive of interactional problems. Including the interaction term in the second block resulted in a statistically significant model that explained a significant additional amount of variance in TSQ interactional problems and suggested a moderation effect.

The Johnson-Neyman technique revealed that ER moderated the negative influence of delusional frequency on TSQ interactional problems only in patients with ER scores (percentiles) between zero and 54.07% (all p < 0.05) suggesting that ER problems attenuated the negative impact of delusional frequency on TSQ interactional problems only in these patients. In persons with higher ratings, the moderation effect was not present.

Discussion

In comparison to controls, patients with psychosis presented problems in both ToM and emotion recognition (ER). Nevertheless, solely problems in ER were related to delusional distress, but not related to other measures of delusions. Problems in ER also had an impact on the quality of the therapeutic alliance and interactional problems viewed by the therapist. Finally, good ER ability reduced the negative impact of delusional frequency on both the quality of the therapeutic relationship and interactional problems during CBT/ST.

The present study is the first study reporting that problems in ER are associated with delusional distress, whereas there were no further associations with other measures of delusions. In particular, problems in recognizing the negative emotions fear and disgust were related with more pronounced delusional distress, whereas problems in recognizing the emotions anger and sadness were not related to delusional distress. Our results suggest that problems in ER (especially problems in recognizing fear and disgust) might not be directly related to the presence of delusions, but enhance the distress associated with them, in line with experimental designs suggesting that problems in ER are more pronounced in stressful situations in patients with psychosis [49]. It is possible that in stressful situations, patients with psychosis are less able to interpret social cues correctly and perform more errors in recognizing emotions and in inferring emotional states of other persons [50, 51] and this might lead to interactional problems and reduced social functioning [23]. Thus, it might be useful to further investigate using longitudinal assessments whether problems in ER - although not directly associated with the intensity of delusions - might increase delusional distress. In addressing this question, it is highly important to measure ER problems with regard to specific negative emotions, as our results demonstrate that problems in recognizing the emotions fear and disgust are related to delusional distress, whereas problems in recognizing anger and sadness were not related to more pronounced delusional distress. If longitudinal associations between problems in ER and specific negative emotions and delusional distress are further established, ER problems should then be included as one of the cognitive factors involved in the development and maintenance of delusional distress in theoretical models.

The fact that we did not find an association between ToM problems and any measure of delusions nor delusional frequency / distress is in line with findings of two meta-analyses [14, 18] and several other studies that did not report an association between ToM and delusions [8, 52, 53], whereas negative symptoms and symptoms of disorganisation are more constantly associated with ToM problems [14]. One explanation might be that ToM problems are less pronounced in patients with delusions in comparison to patients with negative or disorganized symptoms (e.g. [54, 55], see Spronghorst et al. for a review on the literature of subgroup comparisons [56]) and thus, harder to assess using typical ToM paradigms that sometimes lack ecological validity [57]. Interestingly, all studies that used more ecologically valid ToM assessments such as movies of social situations (e.g. the Movie of Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC) [58] or the Movie Task of social situations [20]), found associations between ToM problems and more pronounced general and persecutory delusions in patients with psychosis [59].

An additional limitation of current ToM paradigms is the fact that they often measure ToM in a wright-or-wrong format and thus investigate solely reduced ToM abilities/undermentalizing in patients with psychosis, whereas Abu-Akel [60, 61] suggested that patients with delusion rather present problems in overmentalizing / “hypermentalizing” mental states of other persons, defined as Hyper-ToM [58, 62]. First studies addressed the question of associations between Hyper-ToM and delusions in children with psychotic experiences and normal controls [63, 64] and patients with psychosis [58] and found evidence of an association. Thus, Hyper-ToM rather than undermentalizing might play an important role in the formation and maintenance of delusions and should be investigated in future studies.

Concluding, future studies that address the question of associations between ToM problems and delusions might be well-advised to use tasks with more pronounced ecological validity, for example, ToM assessment using videos, virtual reality (Virtual Assessment of Mentalising Ability (VAMA) [65] or investigating ToM problems in real-life using the experience sampling method [66]. Nevertheless, if there are still no associations between problems in Hyper-ToM and delusions, theoretical models correctly excluded ToM as one of the important cognitive factors involved in the formation and maintenance of delusions or positive symptoms [12, 13].

Our study is the first to report that therapists who treated patients with problems in ER (especially problems in recognizing the emotions disgust and fear) perceived more pronounced interactional problems in these patients. In addition, therapists who treated patients with problems in ER (especially in recognizing the emotions fear, anger, sadness and disgust) rated the quality of the therapeutic relationship more negatively. Our findings are partly in line with the study of Jung and colleagues [29] who reported an association between patients’ ratings on the quality of the therapeutic alliance and ToM problems, but no associations between therapists’ ratings and ToM problems, but the size of our study sample enabled us to detect associations of medium and small effect size. Nevertheless, it has to be taken into account, that our results were not pre-registered, but obtained in an exploratory analysis of a randomized-controlled therapy trial, thus, careful replication of our results should be performed, especially in light of the current replication crisis in psychology (see [67] for a review)). If our results are successfully replicated and patients’ problems in ER influence the relationship with an empathetic and highly skilled therapist, it can be assumed that their problems in ER also have a negative impact on other social interactions in their daily life, as suggested by several other studies that directly addressed this question [21, 22].

In addition, we could provide evidence for the clinically important negative impact of delusional frequency on both the therapeutic relationship and interactional problems viewed by the therapist. Again, it is important to note that these results were obtained in an exploratory analysis and thus are in need of careful replication. If our findings are replicated in longitudinal pre-registered assessments, they suggest that delusional frequency negatively affects social interactions (the therapeutic interactions) and thus may also partly contribute to the association between delusions and lower social functioning [68, 69], negative family atmosphere [70], more pronounced loneliness [71, 72] and social exclusion [73, 74].

Further, preserved ER abilities might protect patients from the negative influence of their delusions on the quality of the therapeutic relationship and interactional problems, as a statistically significant moderation effect occurred. Interestingly, the interaction was most pronounced in patients with severe ER problems: in this subgroup, problems in ER had a specific negative influence on both the therapeutic relationship and interactional problems. Our results are partly supported by a second study that addressed the impact of ToM on the association between delusions and self-rated social functioning [31]. Their results also suggest that preserved ToM abilities moderated the relationship between persecutory delusions and self-rated social functioning [31]. The fact that we did not find a similar moderation effect between ToM and the quality of the therapeutic relationship could be explained by different ToM assessments: the study used the Hinting task [54]) that is based on verbal descriptions of social situations, we used a picture sequencing test based on comics. To some degree, our findings expand on their results, as we used therapists’ ratings of the therapeutic alliance as a direct measure of social functioning instead of self-ratings. Again, if our exploratory findings can be replicated and positive ER skills are a protecting factor against the negative influence of delusions on the therapeutic relationship, patients’ ER abilities might also influence the effectiveness of CBT for psychosis, as a positive therapeutic relationship is closely related to the effectiveness of CBT [28, 75, 76]. Interestingly, one study indeed found general ToM abilities (including ER) to moderate change in positive symptoms in CBT [77]. Thus, interventions that improve ER and ToM abilities might be beneficial in order to improve the therapeutic relationship and, further, the effectiveness of CBT.

ER and ToM problems in patients with psychosis can also be viewed in a broader perspective as parts of patients’ more general problems in their metacognitive capacities. Metacognition has been defined as “cognition about cognition” by Flavell [78] and also discussed as an important part of social cognition [79]. Both ToM and ER are important parts of metacognition, in combination with self-reflectivity (comprehension of one’s own mental state), decentration (the ability to from a complex representation of the world) and mastery, the ability to use information of one’s own and other mental states in respond to and to solve social and psychological problems [80].

In comparison to controls, patients with psychosis were found to present problems in almost all parts of metacognition (see Lysaker et al. [80] for a review on metacognition in schizophrenia). Metacognitive abilities in patients with psychosis are closely linked to a positive therapeutic relationship viewed by patients in CBT (mastery [81]) and also with positive outcome in Cognitive Remediation therapy (learning potential: [82]). Thus, as ER is an important part of metacognition, it is plausible that we also found a link between problems in ER and a less favourable therapeutic relationship and interactional problems viewed by the therapist, as ER can be viewed as one part of metacognitive mastery that was also found to be linked to a positive therapeutic relationship [81]. Thus, the association between problems in ER and a less favourable therapeutic relationship could be moderated by general metacognitive deficits in patients with psychosis. Therapists might perceive these deficits during the first therapeutic sessions and these problems might influence the therapeutic relationship.

For example, patients with deficits in ER and metacognition might present problems in their metacognitive self-reflection that could become visible in diagnostic sessions, as they might not be able to talk about their individual thoughts and emotions in specific situations. They also might present problems in understanding the basic cognitive model that consists of relations between individual perceptions, thoughts, emotions and behaviour [83] due to their problems in self-reflection. Further, patients might also present problems in decentration and thus might not be able to form a complex representation of the world that is important in therapy in order to solve personal and interpersonal problems, e.g. due to their well-known jumping-to-conclusions-bias [84]. Finally, patients’ level of mastery in using their information on mental states in order to solve real-world problems might also be reduced.

Concluding, it is possible that the association between poor emotion recognition and the therapeutic relationship/interactional problems viewed by the therapist can be explained by patients’ metacognitive problems. In addition, it is plausible that not only ER abilities but also a positive metacognitive performance might moderate the influence of delusional frequency on the therapeutic relationship and thus, might also be helpful for patients with psychosis in their general social life, as suggested by a study that found metacognitive capacities to mediate the negative influence of neurocognitive deficits on social functioning in patients with psychosis [85]. Thus, future studies will be well-advised to address all aspects of metacognition in patients with psychosis and their influence on the therapeutic relationship.

Our results suggest for therapists of patients with psychosis to take patients’ potential ER problems (and their metacognitive deficits) into account in CBT for psychosis. First, it could be useful to assess patients with regard to their ER abilities before start of therapy. Second, if patients present ER problems, it is important for therapists to make a special effort to improve the therapeutic relationship with these patients. Third, it could be helpful to train ER (and metacognition) in patients with psychosis using specialized interventions from several social cognition trainings in the framework of Cognitive Remediation [86]: the Social Cognition and Interaction Training [87] and the Metacognitive Training (Moritz and Woodward [88]) aim on improving both ToM and ER, whereas the Training of Affect Recognition [41] aims more closely on ER. In general, these trainings were able to enhance both ToM and ER abilities [87, 89,90,91] and their general positive effect on social functioning is large [92, 93]. It is also possible, that an integration in or a combination of these trainings with CBT in order to improve ER might be beneficial.

Strength and limitations

Strengths of the present study include the large sample of patients with psychosis and the detailed assessment of different dimensions of delusions. An additional strength is the longitudinal assessment of the quality of the therapeutic relationship over five sessions.

In interpreting our findings, it should be mentioned that all associations between ToM, ER, delusions and the therapeutic relationship were of small effect size according to Cohen [94]. In addition, it should be noted that solely two of the four scientific hypotheses were pre-specified, whereas all associations between ER, ToM, delusions and the therapeutic relationship were of exploratory nature. Thus, the question of associations between ToM, ER and the therapeutic relationship and the moderation effect require an additional careful replication study.

In addition, while our moderation model and the mode of assessment (ER and delusions were assessed before the start of therapy) suggest a causal association between problems in ER and delusional frequency and also implicate that positive ER abilities moderate the association between delusional frequency and the therapeutic relationship, we cannot rule out the possibility that a low quality of the therapeutic relationship and interactional problems were influenced by other factors, e.g. common therapeutic factors such as therapists’ empathy, expertness, attractiveness and trustworthiness [95, 96] and thus, it is also possible that an unfavourable therapeutic relationship might lead to more pronounced delusions in patients with psychosis. Thus, future studies should focus on symptom change, ER and the therapeutic relationship using multiple assessments in order to address the question whether more pronounced ER problems cause more pronounced delusional frequency/distress and a less favourable therapeutic relationship (or vice versa).

Finally, it should be mentioned that the patients in the present study were patients interested in participating in a therapy trial who might present better general cognitive and social functioning and less pronounced problems in their social cognition. Nevertheless, as the moderation effect occurred predominantly in patients with lower ER skills, a potential selection bias might not influence the generalisation of our exploratory results.

Conclusions

Several important conclusions can be drawn from our results. First, problems in ER were related to enhanced delusional distress in patients with psychosis. In addition, patients’ deficits in ER had a negative impact on the quality of the relationship viewed by their therapists. Nevertheless, positive ER in patients with psychosis moderated (reduced) the negative impact of delusions on the therapeutic relationship. Thus, improving ER might be a way to improve the quality of the therapeutic relationship and potentially the effectiveness of CBT or other interventions for patients with psychosis.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

CBT:

Cognitive Behavior Therapy.

CBTp:

Cognitive Behavior Therapy for positive symptoms of psychosis.

DSM-IV:

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition.

ER:

Emotion Regulation.

fMRI:

functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging.

MWT-B:

Mehrfachwahlwortschatztest (German verbal intelligence test).

PANSS:

Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale.

PFA:

Pictures of Facial Affect Test.

PSQ:

Patient Session Questionnaire.

PSYRATS:

Psychotic Symptoms Rating Scale.

ST:

Suppportive Therapy.

ToM:

Theory of Mind.

TSQ:

Therapist Session Questionnaire.

References

  1. Premack D, Woodruff G. Does the chimpanzee have a ‘theory of mind’? Behav Brain Sci. 1978;4:515–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Adolphs R. The neurobiology of social cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2001;11(2):231–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Green MF, Leitmann DI. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:670–2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Frith CD. The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2009;109(1–3):1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bora E, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairments in first-episode psychosis, individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2013;144(1):31–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Harrington L, Langdon R, Siegert R, Mc CJ. Schizophrenia, theory of mind, and persecutory delusions. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2005;10:87–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Greig T, Bryson GJ, Bell MD. Theory of mind performance in schizophrenia: diagnostic, symptom and neuropsychological correlates. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2004;192:12–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Bentall RP, de Sousa P, Varese F, Wickham S, Sitko K, Haarmans M, et al. From adversity to psychosis: pathways and mechanisms from specific adversities to specific symptoms. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2014;49(7):1011–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Preti A, Cella M. Paranoid thinking as a heuristic. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2010;4(3):263–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Garety P, Bebbington P, Fowler D, Freeman D, Kuipers E. Implications for neurobiological research of cognitive models of psychosis: a theoretical paper. Psychol Med. 2007;37(10):1377–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Freeman D, Garety P. Advances in understanding and treating persecutory delusions: a review. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2014;49(8):1179–89.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Garety P, Freeman D. The past and future of delusion research: from the inexplicable to the treatable. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203:327–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Ventura J, Wood RC, Hellemann GS. Symptom domains and neurocognitive functioning can help differentiate social cognitive processes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(1):102–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Mitchell RLC, Phillips LH. The overlapping relationship between emotion perception and theory of mind. Neuropsychologia. 2015;70:1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kohler CG, Walker JB, Martin EA, Healey KM, Moberg PJ. Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizphrenia Bulletin. 2010;36(5):1009–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Chan RC, Li H, Cheung EF, Gong QY. Impaired facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178(2):381–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Ventura J, Wood RC, Jimenez AM, Hellemann GS. Neurocognition and symptoms identify links between emotion recognition and emotion processing in schizophrenia: meta-analytic findings. Schizophr Res. 2013;151(0):78–84.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Gaweda L, Staszkiewicz M, Balzan R. The relationship between cognitive biases and psychological dimensions of delusions: the importance of jumping to conclusions. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2017;56:51–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Mehl S, Rief W, Mink K, Lüllmann E, Lincoln TM. Social performance is more closely associated with theory of mind and autobiographical memory than with psychopathological symptoms in clinically stable patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178(2):276–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Gur RE, Kohler CG, Ragland JD. Flat affect in schizophrenia: relation to emotion processing and neurocognitive measures. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32:279–87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Riehle M, Mehl S, Lincoln TM. The specific social costs of expressive negative symptoms in schizophrenia; reduced smiling predicts interactional outcome. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2018;138(2):133–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Fett AK, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez MD, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):573–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Ventura J, Hellemann GS, Thames AD, Koellner V, Nuechterlein KH. Symptoms as mediators of the relationship between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2009;113(2–3):189–99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Martin DJ, Garske JP, Davis MK. Relation of the therapeutic alliance with outcome and other variables: a meta-analytic review. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68(3):438–50.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Horvath AO, Del Re AC, Flückinger C, Symonds D. Alliance in individual psychotherapy. In: Norcross JC, editor. Psychotherapy relationships that work evidence-based responsiveness. 2nd ed. Oxford: University Press; 2011. p. 25–69.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  27. Flückinger C, Del Re AC, Wampold BE, Symonds D, Horvath AO. How central is the alliance in psychotherapy? A multilevel longitudinal meta-analysis. J Couns Psychol. 2012;59:1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Shattock L, Berry K, Degnan A, Edge D. Therapeutic alliance in psychological therapy for schizophrenia and related psychosis: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2017;25:60–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Jung E, Wiesjahn M, Lincoln TM. Negative, not positive symptoms predict the early therapeutic alliance in cognitive behavioral therapy for psychosis. Psychother Res. 2013;24(2):171–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Wittorf A, Jakobi-Malterre UE, Bechdolf A, Mueller BW, Sartory G, Wagner M, et al. The influence of baseline symptoms and insight on the therapeutic alliance early in the treatment of schizophrenia. Eur Psychiatry. 2009;24:259–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Phalen PL, Dimaggio G, Popolo R, Lysaker P. Aspects of theory of mind that attenuate the relationship between persecutory delusions and social functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2017;65:65–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Klingberg S, Wittorf A, Meisner C, Wölwer W, Wiedemann G, Herrlich J, et al. Cognitive behavioural therapy versus supportive therapy for persistent positive symptoms in psychotic disorders: the POSITIVE study, a multicenter, prospective, single-blind, randomised controlled clinical trial. Trials. 2010;11:123.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Wittchen HU, Wunderlich U, Gruschwitz S, Zaudig M. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview fuer DSM-VI (SKID). Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Kay SR, Opler LA, Fiszbein A. Positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS). New York: Multi Health Systems Inc.; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Lehrl S. Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest MWT-B. Fünfte unveränderte Auflage. Spitta Verlag: Balingen; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Walter H, Adenzato M, Ciaramidaro A, Enrici I, Pia L, Bara BG. Understanding intentions in social interaction: the role of the anterior paracingulate cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(10):1854–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Walter H, Ciaramidaro A, Adenzato M, Vasic N, Ardito RB, Erk S, et al. Dysfunction of the social brain in schizophrenia is modulated by intention type: an fMRI study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009;4(2):166–76.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Monke S, Erk S, Schnell K, Schütz C, Romanczuk-Seiferth N, Grimm O, et al. Further evidence for the impact of a genome-wide-supported psychosis risk variant in ZNF804A on the theory of mind network. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(5):1196–205.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologist Press; 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Woelwer W, Frommann N, Halfmann S, Piaszek A, Streit M, Gaebel W. Remediation of impairments in facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: efficacy and specificity of a new training program. Schizophr Res. 2005;80:295–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Frommann N, Streit M, Woelwer W. Remediation of facial affect recognition impairments in patients with schizophrenia: a new training program. Psychiatry Res. 2003;117:281–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Haddock G, McCarron J, Tarrier N, Faragher EB. Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the psychotic symptom rating scales (PSYRATS). Psychol Med. 1999;29(4):879–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Woodward TS, Jung K, Hwang H, Yin J, Taylor L, Menon M, et al. Symptom dimensions of the psychotic rating scales in psychosis: a multisite study. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(4):265–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Flückiger C, Regli D, Zwahlen D, Hostettler S, Caspar F. Der Berner Patienten- und Therapeutenstundenbogen 2000. Z Klin Psychol Psychother. 2010;39(2):71–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Uttl B. Measurement of individual differences: lessons from memory assessment in research and clinical practice. Psychol Sci. 2005;16(6):460–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Berres M, Zehnder A, Bläsi S, Monsch AU. Evaluation of diagnostic scores with adjustment for covariates. Stat Med. 2008;27(10):1777–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Hayes AF, Preacher KJ. Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2014;67:451–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Bauer DJ, Curran PJ. Probing interactions in fixed and multilevel regression: inferential and graphical techniques. Multivar Behav Res. 2005;40(3):373–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Köther U, Lincoln TM. Moritz S. Psychiatry Research: Emotion perception and overconfidence in errors under stress in psychosis; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Lincoln TM, Reumann R, Moritz S. Is there a functional way of responding to paranoid intrusions? Development of the reactions to paranoid thoughts scale. Cogn Neuropsychiatr. 2010;15(4):377–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Freeman D, Stahl D, McManus S, Meltzer H, Brugha T, Wiles N, et al. Insomnia, worry, anxiety and depression as predictors of the occurrence and persistence of paranoid thinking. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012;47(8):1195–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Langdon R, Coltheart M, Ward P. Empathetic perspective-taking is impaired in schizophrenia: evidence from a study of emotion attribution and theory of mind. Cogn Neuropsychiatr. 2006;11(2):133–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Langdon R, Coltheart M, Ward PB, Catts SV. Mentalising, executive planning and disengagement in schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatr. 2001;6(2):81–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Corcoran R, Mercer M, Frith C. Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: investigating theory of mind in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1995;17:5–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Frith CD, Corcoran R. Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1996;26:521–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, van Engeland H. Theory of mind in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;191:8–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Byom LJ, Mutlu B. Theory of mind: mechanisms, methods and new directions. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:413.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Montag C, Dziobek I, Richter IS, Neuhaus K, Lehmann A, Sylla R, et al. Different aspects of theory of mind in paranoid schizophrenia: evidence from a video-based assessment. Psychiatry Res. 2011;186(2–3):203–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Mehl S, Rief W, Lüllmann E, Ziegler M, Kesting ML, Lincoln TM. Are theory of mind deficits in understanding intentions of others associated with persecutory delusions? J Nerv Ment Dis. 2010;198(7):516–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Abu-Akel A, Bailey A. The possibility of different forms of theory of mind impairment in psychiatric and developmental disorders. Psychol Med. 2000;30(3):735–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Abu-Akel A. Impaired theory of mind in schizophrenia. Pragmat Cogn. 1999;7(2):247–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Fretland A, Andersson S, Sundet K, Andreassen OA, Melle I, Vaskinn A. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: error types and associations with symptoms. Schizophr Res. 2015;162(1–3):42–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Clemmensen L, van Os J, Skovgaard AM, Væver M, Blijd-Hoogewys EM, Bartels-Velthuis AA, Jeppesen P. Hyper-theory-of-mind in children with psychotic experiences. Plos one. 2014;9(11).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  64. Clemmensen L, van Os J, Drukker M, Munkholm A, Rimvall MK, Vaever M, et al. Psychotic experiences and hyper theory-of-mind in preadolescence - a birth cohort study. Psychol Med. 2016;46(1):87–101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Canty AL, Neumann DL, Fleming J, Shum DHK. Evaluation of a newly developed measure of theory of mind: the virtual assessment of mentalising ability. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2015;27(5):834–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Bryant L, Coffey A, Povinelli DJ, Pruett JR. Theory of mind experience sampling in typical adults. Conscious Cogn. 2013;22:697–707.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Shrout PE, Rodgers JL. Psychology, science, and knowledge construction: broadening perspectives from the replication crisis. Annu Rev Psychol. 2018;69:487–510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Schaub D, Brüne M, Jaspen E. G. PF, Bierhoff HW, Juckel G. the illness and everyday living: close interplay of psychopathological syndromes and psychosocial funtioning in chronic schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2011;261(2):85–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Dickson JM, Barsky J, Kinderman P, King D, Taylor PJ. Early relationship and paranoia: qualitative investigation of childhood experiences associated with the development of persecutory delusions. Psychiatry Res. 2016;238:40–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Hesse K, Kriston L, Mehl S, Wittorf A, Wiedemann W, Wölwer W, et al. The vicious cycle of family atmosphere, interpersonal self-concepts, and paranoia in schizophrenia—a longitudinal study. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(6):1403–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Meltzer H, Bebbington P, Dennis MS, Jenkins R, McManus S, Brugha TS. Feelings of loneliness among adults with mental disorder. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2013;48(1):5–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Kimhy D, Delespaul P, Corcoran C, Ahn H, Yale S, Malaspina D. Computerized experience sampling method (ESMc): assessing feasibility and validity among individuals with schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 2006;40(3):221–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Sündermann O, Onwumere J, Kane F, Morgan C, Kuipers E. Social networks and support in first-episode psychosis: exploring the role of loneliness and anxiety. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2014;49(3):359–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Giacco D, Palumbo C, Strappelli N, Catapano F, Priebe S. Social contacts and loneliness in people with psychosis. Compr Psychiatry. 2016;70:190–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Frank AF, Gunderson JG. The role of the therapeutic alliance in the treatment of schizophrenia. Relationship to course and outcome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47:228–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Priebe S, Richardson M, Cooney M, Adedeji O, Mc CR. Does the therapeutic relationship predict outcomes of psychiatric treatment in patients with psychosis? A systematic review. Psychother Psychosom. 2011;80:70–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Lincoln TM, Rief W, Westermann S, Ziegler M, Kesting ML, Heibach E, et al. Who stays, who benefits? Predicting dropout and change in cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2014;216(2):198–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Flavell JH. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive-development inquiry. Am Psychol. 1979;34(10):906–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Green MF, Buck B, Healey K, Harvey PD. The social cognition psychometric evaluation study: results of the expert survey and RAND panel. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:813–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Lysaker P, Vohs J, Minor KS, Irrazaval L, Leonhardt B, Hamm JA, et al. Metacognitive deficits in schizophrenia: presence and association with psychosocial outcome. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2015;203:530–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Davies LW, Eicher AC, Lysaker P. Metacognition as a predictor of the therapeutic alliance over 26 weeks of psychotherapy in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2011;129:85–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Tas C, Brown EC, Esen-Danaci A, Lysaker P, Bruene M. Intrinsic motivation and metacognition as predictors of learning potential in patients with remitted schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46:1086–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Beck JS. Cognitive behavior therapy: basics and beyond. New York: Guilford; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Ross RM, Mc Kay R, Coltheart M, Langdon R. Jumping to conclusions about the beads task? A meta-analysis of delusional ideation and data-gathering. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(5):1183–91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Lysaker P, Shea AM, Buck KD, Dimmagio G, Procacci M, Salvatore G, et al. Metacognition as a mediator of the effects of impairment in neurocognition on social functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Acta Psychologica Scandinavica. 2010;122:405–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Wykes T, Huddy V, Cellard C, McGurk SR, Czobor P. A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect sizes. Am J Psychiatr. 2011;168(5):472–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Combs DR, Adams SD, Penn DL, Roberts D, Tiegreen J, Stem P. Social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: preliminary findings. Schizophr Res. 2007;91(1–3):112–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Moritz S, Woodward TS. Metacognitive training in schizophrenia: from basic research to knowledge translation and intervention. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20(6):619–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Kowalski J, Pankowski D, Lew-Starowicz M, Gawęda L. Do specific metacognitive training modules lead to specific cognitive changes among patients diagnosed with schizophrenia? A single module effectiveness pilot study. Psychosis. 2017;9(3):254–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Grant N, Lawrence M, Preti A, Wykes T, Cella M. Social cognition interventions for people with schizophrenia: a systematic review focussing on methodological quality and intervention modality. Clin Pschol Rev. 2017;56:55–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Vaskinn A, Lovgren A, Egeland MK, Feyer FK, Ostefjells T, Andreassen OA, et al. A randomized controlled trial of training of affect recognition (TAR) in schizophrenia showing lasting effects for theory of mind. 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Kurtz MM, Richardson CL. Social cognitive training for schizophrenia: a meta-analytic investigation of controlled research. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(5):1092–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Kurtz MM, Gagen E, Rocha NBF, Machado S, Penn DL. Comprehensive treatments for social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a critical review and effect-size analysis of controlled studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016;43:80–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Cohen D. Statistical power for the social sciences. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Evans-Jones C, Peters E, Barker C. The therapeutic relationship in CBT for psychosis: client, therapist and therapy factors. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2009;37(5):527–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Jung E, Wiesjahn M, Rief W, Lincoln TM. Perceived therapist genuineness predicts therapeutic alliance in cognitive behavioural therapy for psychosis. Br J Clin Psychol. 2015;54:34–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Gudrun Sartory, one of the initiators and principal investigators of the present study, sadly passed away in 2015. We owe much to her leadership, optimism, and lasting support. We would like to thank Jürgen Brinkmeyer for programming the ER task. We would like to thank all participants in the study and all members of the POSITIVE study: S. Baal, J. Berning, S. Beulen, G. Buchkremer, A. Büch, B. Conradt, Y. Eikenbusch, W. Gaebel, A. Gawronski, J. Güttgermans, A. Herold, U. Jakobi-Malterre, I. Lengsfeld, W. Maier, K. Platt, B. Pohlmann, A. Rotarska-Jagiela, S. Sickinger, H. Smoltczyk, S. Unsöld, A. Vogeley, A. Witt, L. Zipp.

Funding

This work was supported by grants awarded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF: 01GV0618, 01GV0620). The study was part of the BMBF research program “Research Networks on Psychotherapy.” The German Federal Ministry of Education and Research was neither involved in the design of the study, the data collection, the data analysis, in the interpretation of data nor in writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AB, JH, TK, SK, BM, GW, AW, WW and MW designed the study and wrote the protocol and the trial proposal, SM, A-CS and ML performed the statistical analysis, SM and A-C S managed the literature search and wrote the first draft of the manuscript, MW, KH and DS co-wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephanie Mehl.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All participants were informed about the assessment and gave written informed consent. In case of a legal guardian, the patients and the guardian were informed about the assessment and both the patient and the guardian gave written informed consent. The ethics committees at the six centres’ medical faculties (University of Bonn, Cologne, Duesseldorf, Frankfurt, Tuebingen and Duisburg-Essen) approved the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

SM, AB, KH, SK, TK, AW and WW have published manuals/ chapters of textbooks and presented workshops and supervision in CBT (all) and cognitive remediation (WW) for psychosis. All authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mehl, S., Hesse, K., Schmidt, AC. et al. Theory of mind, emotion recognition, delusions and the quality of the therapeutic relationship in patients with psychosis – a secondary analysis of a randomized-controlled therapy trial. BMC Psychiatry 20, 59 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12888-020-2482-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12888-020-2482-z

Keywords