Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Qualitative evaluation of a preventive intervention for the offspring of parents with a history of depression

Abstract

Background

Meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials suggest that psychological interventions to reduce children’s risk of depression are effective. Nevertheless, these effects are modest and diminish over time. The Medical Research Council recommends a mixed-methods approach to the evaluation of complex interventions. By gaining a more thorough understanding of participants’ perspectives, qualitative evaluations of preventive interventions could improve their efficacy, longevity and transfer into clinical practice.

Methods

18 parents and 22 children who had received a 12-session family- and group-based cognitive-behavioural intervention to prevent youth depression as part of a randomised controlled trial took part in semi-structured interviews or a focus group about aspects which had been perceived as helpful, elements they were still using after the intervention had ended, and suggestions they had for improving the intervention.

Results

The chance to openly share and discuss their experiences of depression within and between families was considered helpful by both children and parents. Children benefitted the most from learning coping strategies for dealing with stress and many still used them in everyday life. Parents profited mostly from increasing positive family time, but noted that maintaining new routines after the end of the intervention proved difficult. Participants were generally content with the intervention but commented on how tiring and time consuming it was.

Conclusions

Managing parents’ expectations of family-based interventions in terms of their own mental health needs (versus those of their children) and leaving more room for open discussions may result in interventions which are more appealing to participating families. Increasing intervals between sessions may be one means of improving the longevity of interventions.

Trial registration

The original RCT this evaluation is a part of was registered under NCT02115880.

Peer Review reports

Background

The role of parental depression as a risk-factor for youth depression

One of the most detrimental risk factors for depression in children and adolescents is parental depression [1, 2]. Compared to children of parents who have never experienced mental illness, offspring of parents who have been depressed have a two- to threefold greater risk for major depressive disorder [3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10]. This equates to a 50% risk for developing depression by the age of 20 in children with one parent who has been depressed, which increases to 70% if both parents have experienced depression [11,12,13,14]. Episodes of depression during childhood and adolescence are not only associated with short-term negative outcomes such as negative educational achievement and adverse social relationships [2, 15], but are also strongly associated with depression, substance misuse and suicidal behaviour later in life [16]. Although effective treatments for adolescent depression do exist [17], the personal, social and economic burden of depression [18] means that developing effective preventive interventions for the offspring of parents with depression is a major public health priority [1]. Meta-analyses demonstrate that youth depression is indeed preventable [19, 20], although the evidence base for interventions for offspring of parents with depression specifically is less well established (see Loechner et al. [21] for a review of preventive interventions for children of parents with depression).

Preventive interventions for the offspring of parents with depression

A number of biological (e.g. genetic), psychological (e.g. learnt maladaptive coping strategies for dealing with stress) and environmental (e.g. exposure to parental conflict) factors are proposed to underlie the elevated risk of depression in offspring of parents with depression [22]. These potential mechanisms have formed the basis for a number of preventive interventions [23,24,25,26,27] which contain one or more of three ‘key ingredients’: i) psychoeducation about the symptoms, cause, and course of their parents’ depression aims to increase children’s sense of security and control and reduce their sense of anxiety about and responsibility for their parents’ wellbeing, ii) teaching adaptive coping strategies (acceptance, distraction, positive thinking, positive activities) for dealing with stressful situations, and iii) parental training designed to reduce the negative impact of parental depression on the child. These preventive interventions have been evaluated in randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and together, they show small to moderate effects on the onset of depression (RR = 0.56; NNT = 4.28), at least in the short-term [21]. However, the relative benefits of the three key elements are unclear, and there remains a need to further develop these interventions to improve their efficacy and longevity. In addition, little is known about how transferable these interventions are from the research setting to clinical practice.

The “GuG auf” intervention

The “Raising Healthy Children” intervention developed by Compas and colleagues [24, 28] was translated and adapted to German culture (“GuG auf - Gesund und Glücklich aufwachsen!”) and is currently being evaluated in an ongoing randomized controlled trial [29]. The group- and family-based programme offers eight weekly and four monthly booster sessions for four to five families, including homework assignments. It combines psychoeducation for the whole family (first three sessions plus booster sessions) with stress coping strategies for children and positive parenting skills for parents in five additional separate sessions. Sessions last two hours and are conducted by qualified study members (trainee psychiatrists, psychologists, doctoral students) who are trained and supervised by a post-doctoral researcher. For this study, sessions took place in the afternoon, at the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry at the University Hospital Munich, Germany.

The benefits of qualitative research

Existing evaluations of prevention programmes for the offspring of parents with depression have largely been conducted in the form of controlled trials. The RCT is often considered to be the “gold standard” in the assessment of psychological interventions [30,31,32] and enables observed effects to be attributed to the intervention specifically [33, 34]. However, as Midgley and colleagues [32] identify, RCTs often lack external validity in the context of psychotherapy evaluation [35], resulting in difficulties in translating RCT findings into effective changes in clinical practice [36]. One key element of quantitative research is its standardised and predetermined measures, which promote reliability and validity, but can miss out on issues important to participants that go beyond symptom-relief and were not necessarily expected to be relevant [37]. Midgley et al. [32] describe these issues as easier to penetrate via qualitative methods (e.g. open-ended interviews), which are explorative rather than hypothesis-driven [37].

A mixed-methods approach that combines quantitative and qualitative measures [38] as recommended by the Medical Research Council Guidance for complex interventions [39] is becoming increasingly common in the evaluation of psychotherapy [40,41,42] and depression treatments specifically [32, 43,44,45,46]. Incorporating participants’ individual experiences has already been performed in the evaluation of universal prevention programmes [47,48,49,50]. Since qualitative methods have been deemed particularly suitable for understanding the process behind therapeutic change rather than outcomes [51, 52], they may offer insight into the relative effectiveness of the three key elements of preventions for offspring of parents with depression mentioned above. As far as we are aware, only one qualitative study of preventive interventions for this target group has been conducted. Pihkala & Johansson [53] used qualitative interviews to explore parents’ (but not children’s) motivations for taking part in the psychoeducative Family Talk Intervention [12], as well as factors which were perceived as facilitating prevention success.

The current study

This qualitative study complements quantitative data being collected in an ongoing RCT study of a preventive intervention for the offspring of depressed parents [29] and is as such to be read in the broader mixed-methods context of the evaluation. Forty out of 80 children and parents who had participated in the preventive intervention took part in semi-structured interviews or a focus group about their experiences of the intervention. The key aims of the study were to understand which elements of the intervention participants found particularly helpful and how they thought the intervention could be improved, as well as how well participants managed to transfer what they had learnt into everyday life. This approach was adopted to find a balance between gaining feedback on areas of key interest to the team as well as allowing participants the opportunity to raise additional points and concerns in their own words rather than a survey with a limited range of response options. The study was designed to inform the future development of this intervention specifically as well as future preventive interventions for offspring of parents with depression and their implementation into clinical practice more generally.

Methods

Design

Participants who partook in the intervention (described above) were asked to participate in qualitative interviews or a focus group upon completion of the 15-month intervention period. One-to-one interviews followed a semi-structured schedule (Appendices 1 and 2), which was an adaptation of the topic guide developed for the “TARGET” group at King’s College London (Patrick Clark, personal communication) to explore topics relevant to the process of improving the prevention programme. Additionally, a focus group was conducted in order to discuss the same topics in a group setting with both parents and children. As there were similarities in the content categories emerging from the focus group and interview data, these are presented together. This study is reported in accordance with the COREQ guidelines [54], the checklist is included in Additional file 1.

Participants

In qualitative research power calculations are not appropriate and the required sample size relies on the concept of “data saturation” [54, 55], meaning the point at which no new information is being generated by adding new data. In advance, this was assumed to be the case after around 20 to 30 interviews. However, data saturation wasn’t measured separately.

Families who participated in the preventive intervention consisted of at least one parent with a diagnosed episode of depression according to DSM-IV-TR criteria [56] during the child’s lifetime (either current or in remission) and at least one child aged 8–17 years who had no lifetime history of psychiatric illness. Psychiatric status was assessed using the Diagnostic Interview of Psychiatric Disorders (DIPS [57]) and the Diagnostic Interview of Psychiatric Disorders for children and adolescents (K-DIPS [58]). The participation of the second parent was optional. Of the 25 families who had completed the intervention at the time of data collection, 16 families were invited to participate in an interview or focus group about their experiences of the intervention. They were selected because they had been the first to enroll in the intervention and had completed it four to 13 months prior to this study; the only exception were families taking part in the focus group who took part immediately after their last group session since this was the most practical way of coordinating families. The remaining nine families were not contacted because they had only completed the intervention within the past few weeks and the study was partly designed to assess how able families were to implement the intervention contents into their lives in the mid-term. Fifteen families (18 adults and 22 children) agreed to answer questions about their experiences. This sample (described in more detail in Table 1) includes 17 parents with a history of depression, 22 children (one or two children per family), and one partner that did not suffer from depression. Age ranged from 37 to 54 years for parents and from nine to 17 years for children, with gender distributed relatively equally for both parents and children. All participants were of German nationality (good command of the German language was an inclusion criterion for the intervention) and the vast majority of parents (93%) were living together. T-tests and Chi2-tests revealed that this sample did not differ significantly from the full sample of participants who had received the intervention at the time (N = 80) in any of the variables reported below (all Ps > 0.05).

Table 1 Demographic information about the interview and focus group sample

Procedure

Translated schedules for the parent and child interviews and the focus group can be found in Appendices 1, 2 and 3 respectively. They included the same key topics: Had parents and children learned anything new and if so, how much of that knowledge were they still using in everyday life; how had their reactions to stress changed; how had the topic of depression been addressed in the family before the intervention as opposed to during; which logistic factors facilitated, which hindered prevention success. However, the interview structure remained flexible in order to allow participants to raise additional points and concerns. All participants were informed that these feedback interviews were voluntary and that they could withdraw their answers at any time, but were asked to be as honest as possible to help improve the intervention for future participants.

Interviews

Thirty-five participants were invited to take part in interviews either in person as part of their final assessment session for the RCT (15 months after the study began) or via telephone afterwards. Whenever possible, interviews were conducted face-to-face in the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (25 out of 35 interviews) and where this was not possible via telephone at a time of day suggested by the participant (10 out of 35 interviews). Interviews were conducted by four undergraduate Psychology students, the majority by NC as part of a bachelor’s thesis project. Both parents and children were interviewed individually without anyone else present in the room, with participants sitting at a table across from the interviewer and the audio recording device placed on the table. Participants were asked the same questions from the semi-structured interview schedule in order to ensure the same topics of interest were covered, with the option of sharing any additional experiences that had not directly been addressed at the end of the interview. They were given as much time as needed and encouraged to honestly share everything they considered relevant to the improvement of the intervention. Interviews lasted between ten and 40 min, with the parent interviews typically being longer and more elaborate than those with the children.

Focus group

One focus group was conducted by NC with five additional participants who had not been interviewed (three parents and two children) to discuss the same questions in a group setting. The focus group was included on the assumption that a group discussion rather than a one-on-one interview would allow for disagreement between participants and thus potentially more diverse responses and richer data. The focus group lasted approximately 30 min.

Data analysis

The interviews and focus group were recorded with a voice recorder (with the interviewer additionally taking notes during the interviews), anonymised, and subsequently transcribed verbatim into MS Word. Some of the audio files of the interviews (N = 13/35) were not saved correctly and were analysed based on the interviewer’s written notes (bullet points) alone. Quotes in the Results section are identified as participants’ own words or paraphrased interview notes.

A deductive qualitative content analysis was carried out by one coder, following the three phases of analysis recommended by Elo and Kyngäs [59]. A categorisation matrix which covered all presupposed matters of interest was developed and the data was coded for correspondence with these categories [60] by reading and re-reading all transcripts and interview notes. An unconstrained matrix was used in order to be able to create sub-categories based on participants’ replies within the bounds of the presupposed categories [61, 62]. A coding frame was developed to facilitate coding of interview transcripts using the software NVivo [63]. Only manifest content was analysed, with single interviews as unit of analysis [64]. Final identification of categories was based on consensus discussion between members of the research team (NC, BP, LM). Key categories were then described and illustrated with quotes, and then related to each other and back to the research question. Answers are reported below grouped around those categories, with all quotes translated into English from their German original.

Reflexivity

The process of data analysis is influenced by the researcher’s personal perspective, so a degree of “reflexivity” is required [65]. The majority of interviews were conducted by NC (B.A. in Communications and undergraduate Psychology student at the time) who knew the intervention well and had occasionally delivered sessions. This involvement may have put them in a better position to understand and discuss the content of participants’ feedback but may also have led them to perceive participants’ responses less critically than someone independent to the project. The fact that some families knew NC may have led them to be less honest or equally to have opened up more easily. Having conducted qualitative research as part of a dissertation before, NC did their best to encourage honest feedback and minimise the impact of social desirability.

Results

Across the various broad topics in the interview script, numerous strengths and limitations of the GuG auf! intervention could be identified (Table 2). Where responses differed between parents and children this is described; responses from parents with a history of depression and their partners are reported together. Additionally, summaries of key topics including quotes are provided.

Table 2 Participants’ perspectives on strengths and limitations of the intervention, with N = 18 parents and N = 22 children (N = 40)

General acceptability

Although the most commonly named disadvantage to participating in the intervention was how tiring and time consuming it was (N = 17) due to weekly sessions and homework assignments, participants generally displayed a positive attitude towards the prevention intervention and the effort was deemed to be ultimately worthwhile:

"It was encouraging, I think. It gave me courage, that you shouldn't immediately give up." (Child of family 14, transcribed)

For most of them, the intervention represented a valuable first step in starting a conversation:

"I wouldn’t say we’re all irrevocably super happy with each other, but it definitely paved a way." (Parent of family 5, transcribed)

All parents (N = 18) would recommend the intervention to other families, albeit with some caveats (see Table 2):

"If you've got someone like that [currently in a depressive episode], then better tell them wait, wait a little bit, because they can't take in much (...). I'll be completely honest, if it'd been three or four months later, when my psychiatrist had stabilised me a bit, maybe it would've been better." (Parent of family 9, transcribed).

Six parents praised the interactive character of the intervention as particularly helpful, including role-plays, which increased their active interest in the content. Some participants (N = 8), however, described most of the intervention’s contents as already familiar or self-evident:

"Some advice was simply already, uh, being practised in the families. And if participation's voluntary, then it'll be mostly people who are already dedicated to their children, who don't just not care about their children." (Parent of family 10, transcribed).

First memories

When asked which elements of the intervention they could best recall, both children and parents named the stress coping strategies (N = 13), referred to in the intervention as “A’APP”:

"Even though I admittedly don't always immediately know what it means, there's of course these abbreviations, this A'APP. Things like that just stick." (Parent of family 14, transcribed).

Talking about depression

Before the intervention

Relatively few children were completely unaware of their parent’s depression before taking part in the intervention (N = 6). Over half of them (N = 14) said they were definitely aware before but often did not know much specifically and could not recall explicitly talking about it with their parents.

Five parents expressed reluctance to bring up the topic with their children on their own or in detail because of the potential impact it would have on their children:

"Before, you actually, I mean for my part tended to keep it secret, because you didn't want to put a strain on the child." (Parent of family 5, transcribed).

After the intervention

One aspect of the intervention that was perceived to be very helpful by both parents and children was the chance to exchange experiences with other affected families (N = 10):

"Because you hear a lot about, well, this percentage of the population feels the same, but you uh, it's something different when those people are sitting in front of me and sharing their everyday experiences." (Parent of family 5, transcribed).

In contrast to parents’ apprehensions (see above), most children (N = 21) perceived talking about their parents’ depression as a positive experience. The parents’ experiences were similarly positive: Nine of them said the intervention had made it easier to broach the subject within the family:

"Simply the, uh, distance to the topic was kind of gone afterwards and that helps of course, yes, I have to admit that helped me an incredible amount." (Parent of family 8, transcribed)

Three said the information coming from outside of the family had helped to establish common knowledge and hear how their children felt about it. A perhaps surprising number of parents (N = 5) quickly brought up the question of guilt as well, explaining that it was a particular relief to have someone else tell their children that their parents’ depression was not their fault:

"Even if they know that already, it’s good if someone else tells them as well, and even more so if they don’t know." (Parent of family 6, transcribed)

Two of them argued with their own family history:

"I remember when I was fourteen, fifteen, I wasn’t aware of how my mother was doing. And if someone had said – which I actually found trivial now – she isn’t this grumpy because of you (…), that probably would have helped me a lot." (Parent of family 5, transcribed)

Children’s knowledge of depression

Ten children said they now knew more than before the intervention about what depression is:

"That depression is curable and that people still, you know, are normal. That they're just not mentally disturbed, numb people now." (Child of family 12, transcribed).

Evaluating new skills

Children’s coping with stress

Eight children said the intervention had helped them cope with stress and helped them to feel more relaxed. Thirteen children said that they could better control or influence their own thoughts one way or the other after the intervention. Two said they generally felt more in control since they had practised acceptance and staying calm:

"I used to think: $h!t! Now I think: Stay calm for now." (Child 1 of family 1, written notes).

Children were a little more sceptical about being able to influence their own feelings, with seven of them saying they could not influence their feelings any better now. Of note, a lot of children, especially the younger ones, seemed to have difficulty differentiating between their thoughts and their feelings.

Parenting skills

Eight parents specifically called being encouraged to make time for more family activities helpful:

"To really take the time for the whole family. And not just like, 'yes, we could sometime', but actually planning it in more detail." (Parent of family 10, transcribed).

Six parents specifically indicated clear parenting rules and practising how to implement them as helpful:

"Being able to try out such a reward system for a bit, or simply implementing certain parenting strategies that I’d heard of before in theory but just never really officially tried out." (Parent of family 5, transcribed)

However, half of the children (N = 11) did not believe their parents had changed anything about their parenting style.

Implementation in everyday life

Ten participants (3 parents and 7 children) said they were able to implement the intervention into everyday life. The majority of the children said they still used some or all of the stress coping strategies (N = 14), predominantly distraction (N = 7). Nine children specifically mentioned the stress coping strategies as their go-to when faced with a stressful situation, and thirteen would recommend them to a friend looking for advice:

"I think that doesn't just help people who, I don't know, have depression, but all people who get stressed sometimes." (Child 1 of family 5, transcribed).

Eight participants said they still used the learned strategies sometimes, but the process was not automatic enough by the time the intervention was over:

"It's all such a shame (...). For some time we kept doing it, but then it kind of slipped away. You somehow forget about it in your daily routine." (Parent of family 13, transcribed).

Logistics

The majority of participants were happy with the size of the group (N = 32), the time slot for their sessions (N = 32), and the location (N = 34). Children were generally content with being able to attend both children-specific sessions and sessions shared with their parents (N = 15).

Some parents, however, suggested changes in future, wishing either for a less intense intervention (N = 6) with less homework (N = 2) or for more information and challenges (N = 3). Four parents admitted they had been very negligent with the homework, one of which stated they could have profited more if they had done all of the homework:

"But I thought, better to continue without the homework than have great intentions and then say, I can’t do it, and quit." (Parent of family 6, transcribed)

Another three would have liked for the intervention to strive to better involve the whole of the family, including the partner who did not actively partake.

Without direct prompting, some parents emphasised the group leaders’ central role to the intervention’s perceived effectiveness, three of which expressed regret that their group leaders had alternated so much and so their group had somewhat lacked consistency.

The focus group addressed the potential of future online content and how participants would feel about it. All participants were happy with printed materials, but would not have minded additional online content.

Discussion

Interventions designed to prevent depression in the children of parents with a history of depression have been largely evaluated in (randomised) controlled trials. Since the effect sizes of these interventions are modest to moderate at best [21], there is a clear need to investigate which aspects of these complex interventions work best and how existing interventions might be modified to address limitations. Qualitative methods may be better placed to explore these issues and to investigate factors which may enhance or impair implementation of these interventions into practice. To complement quantitative data collected from an RCT study of a preventive intervention for children of parents with depression [29], the present study reports on participants’ subjective experiences of their participation in the intervention. Our findings specifically include feedback from both parents and children as most studies to date have neglected the children’s perspective on intervention success, despite evidence that relying on parental reports alone, particularly depressed parents’ reports of their children’s emotional or behavioural problems, is likely to result in distorted data [66].

Interpretation of findings

Acceptability of the intervention appears to be relatively high: All 18 interviewed parents would encourage families in a similar situation to partake in an intervention like this. The fact that the majority of participants had experienced the intervention as helpful appears to be in line with the original RCT investigating the benefits of this particular family group prevention programme [28] as well as meta-analytic results which show significant, albeit small preventive effects in offspring of depressed parents [21]. Further findings of the study are discussed in relation to the three key components of the intervention mentioned in the introduction: psychoeducation, coping strategies and parenting strategies.

The central aspect that was considered helpful by the majority of both children and parents was sharing their experiences of depression within the family (i.e. psychoeducation). After the intervention children felt like they better understood how depression affected their own parents personally and parents felt relieved that the topic was now “out in the open” and no longer a source of discomfort or guilt. This is in line with Beardslee and colleagues [11, 67,68,69] who argue that interventions which enable an open conversation about depression within the family can be enough to help prevent child depression. According to the study by Pihkala and colleagues [53], the inability to explain their depression by themselves and the need for professional support and mediation is common among parents with a history of depression. Importantly, children described discussing their parent’s depression within the family as a distinctly positive experience and did not view it as distressing, but beneficial. This contradicts the frequently reported parental fear of unnecessarily burdening children with details about their mental illness and wanting to protect them from further stress [70, 71], which not only tends to hinder open discourse within the family but also constitutes another hurdle for participation in prevention programmes. Based on these findings, this fear appears to be unfounded.

The learning content that stuck with participants the most were stress coping strategies. The children specifically appeared to benefit from having a toolbox for dealing with stressful situations, many of them citing this as the most helpful aspect of the intervention. Using these strategies in their everyday life, they perceived themselves to be more in charge of their own thoughts and feelings, effectively improving their self-regulation skills. This positive feedback from the children’s perspective adds to previous works in that it suggests that elements of cognitive-behavioural therapy (CBT) may be a valuable contribution to more basic psycho-educative interventions because they make children feel more in control. There was no single coping strategy that was reported as being more or less helpful, although this question was not posed directly to children.

A lot of content regarding parenting strategies (e.g. positive family time) seemed self-evident to some parents, perhaps due to high motivation levels of the selective sample and/or their experience of psychotherapy. Nevertheless, many of them still felt like they had benefited from the intervention by consciously implementing, practicing and repeating certain contents. This implies that behavioural activation played a particularly important role in the intervention.

While most participants were content with the amount of information presented in the intervention and found its structure and regularity helpful, a few would have preferred more and others less. Most agreed, however, that sessions often felt rushed. Indeed, the group leaders often had trouble covering the contents of the manual because parents particularly had a need to talk more intensively about their personal experiences at home. This may reflect the fact that not all of them (although encouraged) were currently receiving psychotherapy and/or the fact that the intervention, delivered as part of an RCT, was heavily manualised. Participants whose group leaders had alternated a lot expressed regret about their group lacking consistency. Focus group participants seemed indifferent towards the option of potential online content in future versions of the intervention.

Conclusions

There are several impulses that could be taken from the current study in order to enhance the implementation of such interventions into practice, to improve their acceptability and longevity, and to guide future research in this field.

Implementing interventions into practice

Parents’ expectations of preventive interventions should be managed appropriately in advance. Although the researchers gave participants a detailed description of what to expect from the intervention prior to study inclusion, the majority of participants described being surprised by the time-consuming nature of the intervention and the amount of active participation required, especially regarding homework. Furthermore, not all parents were aware that the intervention was aimed mainly at supporting the children rather than dealing with the parents’ depression. A better, more precise understanding of what the intervention provides as well as demands could reduce feelings of disappointment or being overwhelmed. Since most parents who explained their motivation for taking part said they did it to protect their children, this could be used to promote the intervention. Intervention promotion could also include quotes from participants who explained how the group setting and being able to share their experiences helped them overcome their initial anxiety and made it easier to address the subject in the family. Crucially, recruitment should make use of the finding that children were not distressed by the confrontation with their parent’s depression, as is often feared by parents, but instead viewed talking about it as a positive experience.

Just as importantly, parents’ current well-being should be taken into consideration when assessing suitability for preventive interventions. Similarly to findings reported by Pihkala and colleagues [53], those parents who had participated shortly after suffering a depressive episode did not feel like they could profit from such an interactive intervention, a finding supported by an RCT study of the same intervention [24]. Simultaneous psychotherapy treatment might also be further encouraged, given the finding that parents in the current study hoped for more time to discuss issues related to their own depression.

Improving acceptability and longevity

Based on the finding that parents expressed differing expectations of the intervention, wished more flexibility from the intervention and considered a lot of contents to be self-evident, interventions like this might generally need to be more flexible regarding the contents they provide. For example, the promotion of regular positive family time was considered to be either particularly helpful or redundant by participants, depending on how established it had already been within families before joining the intervention. Participants’ frustration could be minimized by using the first few sessions to gauge the needs of individual groups and adapt contents to them.

Modifying the frequency of sessions could also help improve long-term gains from the intervention. Several participants expressed regret that a lot of knowledge remained only passive or was “not automatic enough” by the time the intervention ended. Better preparing them for the end of the intervention may be easier if the time between the last few sessions increases step by step, to make the break between “during” and “after” the intervention feel less sudden.

Finally, a lot of participants only mentioned the term “homework” in a negative context and expressed regret or frustration over the amount, but at the same time would have liked more training opportunities and for their progress to persist. Thus, homework might be framed more consistently as “training” and considered a positive learning opportunity. Additionally, the potentially negative impact of fluctuating group leaders on how acceptable the intervention is should not be underestimated.

Future research

The present work has shown that qualitative data can offer valuable additional information about what kind of an impact preventive interventions like this can have on participants’ everyday lives and which elements are perceived as particularly conducive or obstructive to prevention success. Researchers investigating prevention effects should adopt a mixed-methods approach as standard procedure in the future to avoid the “implementation gap” [72] so often associated with strictly quantitative evaluations. It might be worth considering using interviewers that are not part of the project and so have fewer preconceptions about its merit. Lastly, it would be interesting to further investigate any difference in intervention effectiveness and acceptability for families who participated with one parent only compared to both parents taking part in the intervention.

Strengths & Limitations

A strength of this work is its mixed-methods approach, in which quantitative data collected in an RCT are supplemented with qualitative feedback. To our knowledge, it is the first qualitative evaluation of a selective prevention intervention for families with depression that takes into account both the parents’ and the children’s view. The explorative nature of this work allowed us to investigate participants’ perceptions of the strengths and weaknesses of the intervention which might not have been addressed in the quantitative evaluation. Furthermore, the findings have additional implications for future research as well as implementing such interventions into clinical practice. Since effects on mediators and moderators are inconsistent [73, 74] and mechanisms of prevention interventions remain unknown, taking the perspective of the recipients of those interventions is essential in order to increase modest preventive intervention effects [19,20,21]. We were able to collect data from 40 family members in total and also to conduct a focus group in order to garner richer data. However, it is important to keep in mind that due to the qualitative study design, the data are not intended to be representative.

A limitation is the nature of the sample: Despite a family history of depression, all study participants had taken part in a time-consuming intervention. This indicates a particularly motivated sample. In addition, families were mostly German and had a high socio-economic background. Consequently, the intervention might be experienced differently by families from different social and cultural backgrounds, who might have different parenting values and/or financial and time constraints. These families are often those who are neglected regarding psychosocial support and have limited access to mental healthcare [75]. Furthermore, the inclusion of children and adolescents resulted in shorter interviews and focus group and thus less data; it also meant participants could often only be reached by telephone and so many of the interviews were conducted via telephone rather than in person. Another limitation was the structure of the interview schedule, which was not sufficiently oriented toward the younger participants and made it difficult to yield more elaborate replies. Unfortunately, some early interviews were not recorded and the interviewer instead made notes during the interview, resulting in data of a poorer quality.

Finally, we experienced difficulties organising and conducting the focus group. Of the four group families, only two could be present for the focus group due to personal time restrictions such that group discussion was limited.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available since the publication of raw data was not included in the consent forms, but all data and materials are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Notes

  1. At the time the intervention was referred to as “PRODO” (the time of the study trial) rather than “GuG auf”.

Abbreviations

A’APP:

Acceptance, Distraction, Positive Thoughts, Positive Activities (Akzeptanz, Ablenkung, Positive Gedanken, Positive Aktivitäten); mnemonic device to help participants remember the stress coping strategies they learned throughout the programme

CBT:

Cognitive-behavioural therapy

RCT:

Randomised controlled trial

References

  1. World Health Organisation. Prevention of Mental Disorders: Effective Interventions and Policy Options. Summary Report. Geneva: Prevention Research Centre of the Universities of Nijmegen and Maastricht; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Weissman MM, Wickramaratne PJ, Nomura Y, Warner V, Pilowsky D, Verdeli H. Offspring of depressed parents: 20 years later. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1001–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Downey G, Coyne JC. Children of depressed parents: an integrative review. Psychol Bull. 1990;108:50–76.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Hammen C, Burge D, Burney E, Adrian C. Longitudinal study of diagnoses in children of women with unipolar and bipolar affective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47:1112–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Klein DN, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR, Rohde P. A family study of major depressive disorder in a community sample of adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:13–20.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Kovacs M, Devlin B, Pollock M, Richards C, Mukerji P. A controlled family history study of childhood-onset depressive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54:613–23.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Lieb R, Isensee B, Höfler M, Pfister H, Wittchen H-U. Parental major depression and the risk of depression and other mental disorders in offspring: a prospective-longitudinal community study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:365–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Orvaschel H, Walsh-Allis G, Ye W. Psychopathology in children of parents with recurrent depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1988;16:17–28.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Williamson DE, Birmaher B, Axelson DA, Ryan ND, Dahl RE. First episode of depression in children at low and high familial risk for depression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43:291–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1097/00004583-200403000-00010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Rasic D, Hajek T, Alda M, Uher R. Risk of mental illness in offspring of parents with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of family high-risk studies. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:28–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Beardslee WR, Podorefsky D. Resilient adolescents whose parents have serious affective and other psychiatric disorders: importance of self-understanding and relationships. Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145:63–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Beardslee WR, Versage E, Giadstone T. Children of affectively ill parents: a review of the past 10 years. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37:1134–41.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Mattejat F. Kinder depressiver Eltern. In: Braun-Scharm H, editor. Depressionen bei Kindern und Jugendlichen. Stuttgart: Wissenschaftliche Verlagsgesellschaft mbH; 2002. p. 231–45.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Robins LN, Regier DA. Psychiatric disorders in America. New York: Free Press; 1991.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Birmaher B, Ryan ND, Williamson DE, Brent DA, Kaufman J, Dahl RE, et al. Childhood and adolescent depression: a review of the past 10 years. Part I. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35:1427–39. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1097/00004583-199611000-00011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Rudolph KD, Klein DN. Exploring depressive personality traits in youth: Origins, correlates, and developmental consequences. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21:1155–80. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1017/S0954579409990095.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Zhou X, Hetrick SE, Cuijpers P, Qin B, Barth J, Whittington CJ, et al. Comparative efficacy and acceptability of psychotherapies for depression in children and adolescents: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. World Psychiatry. 2015;14:207–22. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/wps.20217.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Mathers C, Fat DM, Boerma JT. The global burden of disease: 2004 update: World Health Organization; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Hetrick SE, Cox GR, Witt KG, Bir JJ, Merry SN. Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), third-wave CBT and interpersonal therapy (IPT) based interventions for preventing depression in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016;8:CD003380. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/14651858.CD003380.pub4.

  20. Stockings EA, Degenhardt L, Dobbins T, Lee YY, Erskine HE, Whiteford HA, et al. Preventing depression and anxiety in young people: a review of the joint efficacy of universal, selective and indicated prevention. Psychol Med. 2016;46:11–26. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1017/S0033291715001725.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Loechner J, Starman K, Galuschka K, Tamm J, Schulte-Körne G, Rubel J, et al. Preventing depression in the offspring of parents with depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Clin Psychol Rev. 2017;:In press. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.11.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Goodman SH, Gotlib IH. Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: a developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychol Rev. 1999;106:458–90.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Beardslee WR, Versage E, Wright EJ, Salt P, Rothberg PC, Drezner K, et al. Examination of preventive interventions for families with depression: evidence of change. Dev Psychopathol. 1997;9:109–30 https://0-www-cambridge-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/core/article/div-class-title-examination-of-preventive-interventions-for-families-with-depression-evidence-of-change-div/A98F5AF5D489CA8FD63F7160C67D8C09.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Compas BE, Forehand R, Keller G, Champion JE, Rakow A, Reeslund KL, et al. Randomized controlled trial of a family cognitive-behavioral preventive intervention for children of depressed parents. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2009;77:1007–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Garber J, Clarke GN, Weersing VR, Beardslee WR, Brent DA, Gladstone TRG, et al. Prevention of depression in at-risk adolescents: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA Psychiatry. 2009;301:2215–24.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Mason WA, Haggerty KP, Fleming AP, Casey-Goldtein M. Family intervention to prevent depression and substance use among adolescents of depressed parents. J Child Fam Stud. 2012;21:891–905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Sanford M, Byrne C, Williams S, Atley S, Ridley T, Miller J, et al. A pilot study of a parent-education Group for Families Affected by depression. Can J Psychiatry. 2003;48:78–86. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/070674370304800203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Compas BE, Forehand R, Thigpen JC, Keller G, Hardcastle EJ, Cole DA, et al. Family group cognitive–behavioral preventive intervention for families of depressed parents: 18- and 24-month outcomes. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2011;79:488–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Platt B, Pietsch K, Krick K, Oort F, Schulte-Körne G. Study protocol for a randomised controlled trial of a cognitive-behavioural prevention programme for the children of parents with depression: the PRODO trial. BMC Psychiatry. 2014;14:263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Harbour R, Miller J. A new system for grading recommendations in evidence based guidelines. Br Med J. 2001;323:334–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Kaptchuk TJ. The double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial: gold standard or golden calf? J Clin Epidemiol. 2001;54:541–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/S0895-4356(00)00347-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Midgley N, Ansaldo F, Target M. The meaningful assessment of therapy outcomes: incorporating a qualitative study into a randomized controlled trial evaluating the treatment of adolescent depression. Psychotherapy. 2014;51:128–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Blackwood B, O’Halloran P, Porter S. On the problems of mixing RCTs with qualitative research: the case of the MRC framework for the evaluation of complex healthcare interventions. J Res Nurs. 2010;15:511–21. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/1744987110373860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Spillane JP, Pareja AS, Dorner L, Barnes C, May H, Huff J, et al. Mixing methods in randomized controlled trials (RCTs): validation, contextualization, triangulation, and control. Educ Assess Eval Account. 2010;22:5–28. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s11092-009-9089-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. McLeod J. The role of qualitative methods in outcome research. In: McLeod J, editor. Qualitative research in counselling and psychotherapy. 2nd ed. London: SAGE Publications; 2011. p. 161–80.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Hollon SD. Randomized clinical trials. In: Norcross JC, Beutler LE, Levant RF, editors. Evidence-based practices in mental health: debate and dialogue on the fundamental questions. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2006. p. 96–105.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Stiles WB, Hurst RM, Nelson-Gray R, Hill CE, Greenberg LS, Watson JC, et al. What qualifies as research on which to judge effective practice? In: Norcross JC, Beutler LE, Levant RF, editors. Evidence-based practices in mental health: debate and dialogue on the fundamental questions. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2006. p. 56–130.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  38. Dures E, Rumsey N, Morris M, Gleeson K. Mixed methods in Health Psychology. J Health Psychol. 2010;16:332–41. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/1359105310377537.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Craig P, Dieppe P, Macintyre S, Michie S, Nazareth I, Petticrew M. Developing and evaluating complex interventions. 2019. www.mrc.ac.uk/complexinterventionsguidance.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Down R, Willner P, Watts L, Griffiths J. Anger management groups for adolescents: a mixed-methods study of efficacy and treatment preferences. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;16:33–52. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/1359104509341448.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Richards A, Ospina-Duque J, Barrera-Valencia M, Escobar-Rincón J, Ardila-Gutiérrez M, Metzler T, et al. Posttraumatic stress disorder, anxiety and depression symptoms, and psychosocial treatment needs in Colombians internally displaced by armed conflict: a mixed-method evaluation. Psychol Trauma Theory Res Pract Policy. 2011;3:384–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Davidson L, Ridgway P, Kidd S, Topor A, Borg M. Using qualitative research to inform mental health policy. Can J Psychiatr. 2008;53:137–44. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/070674370805300303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Barnes M, Sherlock S, Thomas L, Kessler D, Kuyken W, Owen-Smith A, et al. No pain, no gain: depressed clients’ experiences of cognitive behavioural therapy. Br J Clin Psychol. 2013;52:347–64. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/bjc.12021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Buus N, Johannessen H, Stage KB. Explanatory models of depression and treatment adherence to antidepressant medication: a qualitative interview study. Int J Nurs Stud. 2012;49:1220–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2012.04.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Cramer H, Salisbury C, Conrad J, Eldred J, Araya R. Group cognitive behavioural therapy for women with depression: pilot and feasibility study for a randomised controlled trial using mixed methods. BMC Psychiatry. 2011;11:82. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-244X-11-82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Nakimuli-Mpungu E, Wamala K, Okello J, Ndyanabangi S, Kanters S, Mojtabai R, et al. Process evaluation of a randomized controlled trial of group support psychotherapy for depression treatment among people with HIV/Aids in Northern Uganda. Community Ment Health J. 2017;53:1–14. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s10597-017-0129-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Renner W, Platz T, Esmann H, Mills S. Evaluation eines psychologischen Präventionsprogramms zur betrieblichen Gesundheitsförderung: quantitative und qualitative Ergebnisse. Prax Klin Verhal und Rehabil. 2000;13:67–75.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Bühler A, Schröder E, Silbereisen RK. Welche Lebensfertigkeiten fördert ein suchtpräventives Lebenskompetenzprogramm? Quantitative und qualitative Ergebnisse einer schulbasierten Interventionsstudie. Zeitschrift für Gesundheitspsychologie. 2007;15:1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Skryabina E, Morris J, Byrne D, Harkin N, Rook S, Stallard P. Child, teacher and parent perceptions of the FRIENDS classroom-based universal anxiety prevention Programme: a qualitative study. School Ment Health. 2016;8:486–98. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s12310-016-9187-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Scope A, Booth A, Morrell CJ, Sutcliffe P, Cantrell A. Perceptions and experiences of interventions to prevent postnatal depression. A systematic review and qualitative evidence synthesis. J Affect Disord. 2017;210:100–10. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Rubin H, Rubin I. Qualitative interviewing: the art of hearing data. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Smith JA. Qualitative methodology: analysing participants’ perspectives. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 1996;9:417–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Pihkala H, Johansson EE. Longing and fearing for dialogue with children: depressed parents’ way into Beardslee’s preventive family intervention. Nord J Psychiatry. 2008;62:399–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Tong A, Sainsbury P, Craig J. Consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ): a 32-item checklist for interviews and focus groups. Int J Qual Heal Care. 2007;19:349–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Guest G, Bunce A, Johnson L. How many interviews are enough? Field Methods. 2006;18:59–82. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/1525822X05279903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. IV-TR. Washington, D.C.: Author; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Schneider S, Margraf J. Diagnostic Interview for Psychiatric Disorders [Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen]. Berlin: Springer; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Unnewehr S, Schneider S, Margraf J. Kinder-DIPS: Diagnostisches Interview bei Psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. Heidelberg: Springer Medizin; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Elo S, Kyngäs H. The qualitative content analysis process. J Adv Nurs. 2008;62:107–15. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2007.04569.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Polit DF, Beck CT. Nursing Research: Principles and Methods. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Patton MQ. Qualitative evaluation and research methods. Newbury Park: SAGE Publications; 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Sandelowski M. Qualitative analysis: what it is and how to begin. Res Nurs Health. 1995;18:371–5. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/nur.4770180411.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  63. QSR International Pty Ltd. NVivo qualitative data analysis software. 2012. http://www.qsrinternational.com/nvivo-product.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Graneheim U, Lundman B. Qualitative content analysis in nursing research: concepts, procedures and measures to achieve trustworthiness. Nurse Educ Today. 2004;24:105–12. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/J.NEDT.2003.10.001.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Malterud K. Qualitative research: standards, challenges, and guidelines. Lancet. 2001;358:483–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/S0140-6736(01)05627-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Gartstein MA, Bridgett DJ, Dishion TJ, Kaufman NK. Depressed mood and maternal report of child behavior problems: another look at the depression–distortion hypothesis. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2009;30:149–60. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Beardslee WR, Wright EJ, Gladstone TRG, Forbes P. Long-term effects from a randomized trial of two public health preventive interventions for parental depression. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21:703–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Beardslee WR, Gladstone TRG, Wright EJ, Cooper AB. A Family-Based Approach to the Prevention of Depressive Symptoms in Children at Risk: Evidence of Parental and Child Change. Pediatrics. 2003;112:e119 LP–e131 http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/112/2/e119.abstract.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Focht-Birkerts L, Beardslee WR. A Child’s experience of parental depression: encouraging relational resilience in families with affective illness*. Fam Process. 2000;39:417–34. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1545-5300.2000.39403.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Festen H, Schipper K, de Vries SO, Reichart CG, Abma TA, Nauta MH. Parents’ perceptions on offspring risk and prevention of anxiety and depression: a qualitative study. BMC Psychol. 2014;2:17. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/2050-7283-2-17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Stallard P, Norman P, Huline-Dickens S, Salter E, Cribb J. The effects of parental mental illness upon children: a descriptive study of the views of parents and children. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;9:39–52. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/1359104504039767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Britten N. Qualitative research and the take-up of evidence-based practice. J Res Nurs. 2010;15:537–44. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/1744987110380611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Compas BE, Forehand R, Thigpen JC, Hardcastle EJ, Garai E, McKee LG, et al. Efficacy and moderators of a family group cognitive–behavioral preventive intervention for children of parents with depression. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2015;83:541–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Weersing VR, Shamseddeen W, Garber J, Hollon SD, Clarke GN, Beardslee WR, et al. Prevention of depression in at-risk adolescents: predictors and moderators of acute effects. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;55:219–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Perrino T, Beardslee W, Bernal G, Brincks A, Cruden G, Howe G, et al. Toward scientific equity for the prevention of depression and depressive symptoms in vulnerable youth. Prev Sci. 2015;16:642–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to our interns Andrea Hauslbauer, Jakob Neumüller, Sandra Thiele, Lisa-Beerman-Wendel, Melanie Andronie, Jeanette Tamm, Claudia Werner, Theresa Tremmel, Liliana Lelek, Julia Deutlmoser, Leonie Bäumler, Dana Winogradow, Anna Wagner and Markus Koenigs for their support in recruiting families, delivering the intervention, collecting data and translating documents. We are also grateful for the support of our study nurse team. Thank you to Bruce Compas and Emily Hardcastle for their cooperation in adapting the “Raising Healthy Children” intervention and to Patrick Clark for sharing his interview schedule.

Funding

The study is funded by the Bavarian Ministry of Environment, Public Health and Consumer Protection. The funding body played no role in the design of the study or writing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

NC was responsible for recruiting participants, conducting interviews, analysing and interpreting data and preparation of the manuscript. LM contributed to the study design, analysing and interpreting data and preparation of the manuscript. JL, KS, AV, FL, IW, SH, LE and MB recruited participants and delivered the intervention. GSK contributed to the study design. BP contributed to the study design, analysing and interpreting data and preparation of the manuscript. All authors read and critically revised the manuscript and approved its final version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nathalie Claus.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty at the LMU Munich (reference number 3–14) and all participating parents and children provided written informed consent to participate in the interviews and the focus group prior to their inclusion in the study. Data were collected between September 2016 and April 2017.

Consent for publication

Not applicable (no personal data published).

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1.

COREQ Checklist. Checklist for qualitative research papers in order to uphold high research standards and provide transparency for the readers.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Interview schedule for parents (Translated from German original)

Thank you for participating! The interview is going to take 30–45 min. We ask everyone who has participated in the programme for their feedback in order to improve our programme. You can ask at any time if something is unclear.

Similar to the other sessions we are going to make an audio recording of this evaluation so we can transcribe the discussion later on. We are going to remove your names from your feedback and the group leaders are not going to find out who said what, that means that you can be absolutely honest. Things we discuss are, like in other GuG auf sessions, confidential.

Topic guide

  • What do you think about the programme in general?

  • What was helpful or not helpful at all?

  • What do you remember best?

  • Did you learn something new?

  • Did you learn something that you currently use or that you would use during stressful situations in the future?

  • How could these strategies help to improve the situation? Was there something you would have liked to talk about that we did not cover?

  • Would you recommend the programme to a friend?

  • What advantages and disadvantages did you experience as a participant of the programme?

  • Was the size of the group comfortable?

  • Did your daughter/son know that depression is an issue in your family before you participated in the programme?

    • ◦ If not, how was it for you to tell your child about it? Or to participate in the programme?

    • ◦ What was it like to explain your depression to your children?

  • Did you feel like the programme focused on the important things for your family?

  • In your opinion, how much did you get involved in the programme?

  • Are you happy with the amount of information you received during the programme?

  • Do you think that other families should participate in such programmes?

  • Did you like the time and the location for the group sessions?

Appendix 2

Interview schedule for children (Translated from German original)

Thank you for participating! The interview is going to take 30–45 min. We ask every child who participated in our programme to give us feedback. This interview is going to help us to evaluate and to improve our programme. You can ask at any time if something is unclear.

Similar to the other sessions we are going to make an audio recording of this evaluation so we can transcribe the discussion later on. We are going to remove your names from your feedback and the group leaders are not going to find out who said what, that means that you can be absolutely honest. Things we discuss are, like in other GuG auf sessions, confidential.

  • Did you know that your mother/father has suffered from depression before you participated in the programme?

  • If not, how was it for you to hear about it for the first time? (Did it make you feel sad, worried, interested…)?

  • Did your knowledge about depression change?

  • What do you think about the programme? (The conversations, your contributions, workbook, training, etc)

  • What helped you the most/least?

  • What do you remember most?

  • Did you learn something new?

  • Did you learn something that you now use? Or can you imagine that you would use one of the strategies during stressful situations or when you’re feeling sad in the future? How could you improve those?

  • What advice would you give to your friends when they are sad or stressed?

  • Do you cope with stress now in another way than before the programme? If yes, how?

  • Did you tell your friends about the sessions? If yes, what do they think about it?

  • Which advantages and disadvantages did you experience as a participant of the programme?

  • Was the size of the group comfortable?

  • Did you like the time and the location for group sessions?

  • Do you think your parents have changed their parenting behaviour?

  • Would you prefer a group that is only for children or a mixed group with parents?

  • Do you feel like you have better control of your thoughts now? Do you think you have better control of your feelings?

Appendix 3

Focus group schedule (Translated from German original)

Thank you for agreeing to participate in today’s evaluation. We appreciate you taking time to share your experiences with us. This evaluation helps us to find out which parts of GuG aufFootnote 1 worked well and what we should change in the future to support participating families best. Your comments are important because you have passed through the whole programme and you know best what it means to participate in GuG auf.

Similar to the other sessions we are going to record this evaluation on video so we can transcribe the discussion later on. We are going to remove your names from your feedback and the group leaders are not going to find out who said what, that means that you can be absolutely honest. Things we discuss are, like in other GuG auf sessions, confidential.

If there are any questions you don’t want to answer, then you don’t have to – but of course it would be nice to get as much feedback as possible.

So we don’t miss any important feedback, please make sure only one person talks at a time. There is no right or wrong, we just would like to hear your honest opinion. Everyone can answer freely, say things that come to your mind, without a fixed order. Are there any questions before we begin?

Questions / Topics:

  • General feedback – what do you think about GuG auf now at the end of the programme?

  • What was especially helpful for you? What was not helpful at all or even unnecessary?

  • What do you remember best? What is the first thing that comes to your mind when you think of GuG auf?

  • Did you learn something new from GuG auf?

  • Is there something that you learnt in GuG auf that you currently use in everyday life when you feel stressed or in a bad mood?

  • Were there any issues that were not discussed but that you would have liked to talk about? Was there anything missing?

  • Did you have the impression that the programme focused on the important issues in your family?

  • Would you recommend the programme to a friend?

  • What were the advantages of taking part in GuG auf? Were there any disadvantages?

  • What was it like in your family, did you talk about depression with each other before participating in GuG auf? Was it different talking about it in the GuG auf sessions as opposed to at home? If yes, how?

  • In your opinion, how intense was your commitment to GuG auf?

  • Were you happy with the amount of information you received through GuG auf? Was it too much/too little?

  • Did you like the group size? Did you like the location? The time of day?

  • We’re thinking about integrating the internet into GuG auf a bit more in the future. For example, filling in the homework online or providing the workbooks online. Do you think this would make sense?

  • Is there anything else you would like to mention that I have not asked you about?

Thank you for your honesty!

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Claus, N., Marzano, L., Loechner, J. et al. Qualitative evaluation of a preventive intervention for the offspring of parents with a history of depression. BMC Psychiatry 19, 290 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12888-019-2273-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12888-019-2273-6

Keywords