Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

The protective role of religiosity against problem gambling: findings from a five-year prospective study

Abstract

Background

Little research has examined the potential protective influence of religiosity against problem gambling; a common addictive behavior, and one with a host of associated negative health and social outcomes. The aims of this study were to examine (1) the potential longitudinal association between religiosity and problem gambling among adults and (2) the potential moderating role of gender on this association.

Methods

Data were from five waves of the Quinte Longitudinal Study (QLS), between 2006 and 2010. Participants were Canadian adults from Belleville, Ontario, Canada (n = 4121). A multiple group (based on gender) latent growth curve analysis was conducted to examine the overall trajectory of problem gambling severity. Two models were tested; the first examined the influence of past-year religious service attendance, and the second examined an overall measure of personal religiosity on the trajectory of problem gambling. The Problem and Pathological Gambling Measure (PPGM) was used as a continuous measure. The Rohrbaugh-Jessor Religiosity Scale (RJRS) was used to assess past-year frequency of religious service attendance and personal religiosity. Religious affiliation (Protestant, Catholic, Atheist/Agnostic, Other, Prefer not to say) was also included in the models.

Results

At baseline, higher frequency of past-year religious service attendance (males: β= −0.54, females: β= −0.68, p < 0.001 for both) and greater overall personal religiosity (males: β= −0.17, females: β= −0.13, p < 0.001 for both) were associated with lower PPGM scores. The moderating effect of gender indicated that the influence of past-year religious service attendance was greater among females (χ2diff(44) = 336.8, p < 0.001); however, the effect of overall religiosity was greater among males (χ2diff(36) = 213.4, p < 0.001). Findings were mixed with respect to religious affiliation. No measures of religiosity or religious affiliation were associated with the overall decline in problem gambling severity.

Conclusions

These findings suggest that religiosity may act as a static protective factor against problem gambling severity but may play a less significant role in predicting change in problem gambling severity over time.

Peer Review reports

Background

A large body of evidence indicates that religion and religious involvement are associated with positive mental health and physical health outcomes, including decreased rates of depression, suicide, and coronary heart disease [1]. There is also research highlighting the protective influence of religiosity against addictive behaviors such as smoking, alcohol and drug use [1, 2]. However, there has been relatively little research which examines the potential protective influence of religiosity against excessive gambling, a common addictive behavior, and one with a host of associated negative outcomes including criminal behavior [3], psychiatric comorbidities [4], and financial issues [5]. The limited amount of research in the field has indicated that religious involvement is inversely related to ever-gambling [6, 7], past-year gambling [8], gambling frequency, and the amount of money gambled [9,10,11].

It is important to note that religiosity is multi-dimensional in nature, and has both a public and private component. For example, attending religious services can be considered a public display or community aspect of religiousness, whereas the extent to which someone internalizes their faith and uses it to guide their life and decision-making can be considered the private component. In addition to public and private components of religiosity, religious affiliation represents another dimension of religiosity that is important to understand in the context of gambling. Religious traditions tend to have diverse ethical codes of conduct with regards to gambling. For example, although gambling is not explicitly prohibited in the Bible, conservative and mainline Protestant groups interpret its teachings as such. Within the Islamic tradition, gambling is explicitly prohibited according to the Quran. On the other hand, there are religious traditions that do not reject gambling behavior categorically, such as the Catholic and Jewish traditions. Overall, religious affiliation and its codes of conduct have the potential to shape cultural norms, beliefs, and ultimately behavior.

One major methodological limitation of studies examining the influence of various dimensions of religiosity on gambling is that most are cross-sectional, and unable to examine these associations over time. To our knowledge, only one study has tested the longitudinal association between religiosity and gambling [6]. This study used data from the US National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health, and found that religiousness during adolescence predicted aspects of gambling behavior in young adulthood. Additional longitudinal research is needed to assess the potential protective influence of religiosity on subsequent problem gambling behaviors in other jurisdictions and among different age cohorts.

Moreover, despite evidence showing that men and women differ in terms of the occurrence, prevalence, correlates, and consequences of their addictive behaviors [12, 13], the role of gender has not yet been explicitly examined in studies related to gambling and religiosity. It has been hypothesized that religiosity is influenced by underlying personality traits, namely risk aversion [14, 15]. This line of thinking posits that those who are more risk-averse, are also more likely to be religious. This notion has been used to explain consistent gender differences across various dimensions of religiosity indicating that women tend to be more religious overall [14, 15]. However, a recent study found no evidence supporting the idea that risk-aversion explains the association between religion and gambling [6].

The primary goal of the current study was to examine the potential protective influence of religious service attendance and overall religiosity on problem gambling severity over time, among a sample of Canadian adults. Specific aims were to: 1) examine the trajectory of problem gambling severity, and 2) examine the potential influence of religious service attendance and personal religiosity on problem gambling severity both at baseline and over time, and 3) examine the potential moderating role of gender on these associations.

Methods

Sample and procedure

Data were from the Quinte Longitudinal Study (QLS) [16], the primary aim of which was to help develop an etiological model of problem gambling. Ethical approval was provided by the Human Subject Research Committee at the University of Lethbridge. A cohort of 4121 adults from Ontario, Canada were assessed yearly over a five-year period (2006 to 2011).

Recruitment was conducted via random digit telephone dialing from a pool of numbers with area codes and prefixes estimated to be within 70 km of the city of Belleville in Ontario, Canada. The response rate in the QLS was 21.3%, a similar value to those obtained in research with a similar focus from the same country e.g., [17]. The retention rate for the QLS was 93.9%, an exceptionally high value in large scale longitudinal research of this nature. Two samples were recruited: a ‘general population’ sample (n = 3065) and an ‘at risk’ sample for problem gambling (n = 1056). The purpose of recruiting the ‘at risk’ subsample was to ensure that there were a sufficient number of people in the cohort who became problem gamblers during the course of the study. For a respondent to be considered ‘at-risk’, they had to indicate one or more of the following: (1) spending $10 or more per month on lottery, instant win tickets, bingo, casino table games, or games of skill against other people; (2) playing either slot machines or betting on horse racing in the past year; (3) an intention to gamble at a new slots-at-racetrack facility that was scheduled to be built sometime in the next few years in the area.

If the person agreed to participate in the survey, they were sent an email with a link to the online questionnaire or booked into a time slot at the program office where they completed the survey on a computer on site. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants prior to completing the online survey, each year. A total of 69.5% opted to complete the survey online for the first assessment, with this proportion steadily increasing to 90.0% for the final assessment. A small percentage of people completed a paper and pencil version of the survey because of their unfamiliarity with computers (1.2% to 1.9% depending on the survey year). Further methodological details can be found elsewhere [18].

The focus of the QLS was to examine changes in gambling over time, thus it was not necessary that the sample be representative of the Ontario or Canadian population, only that it contained a diverse range of gamblers. Nonetheless, the demographic profile of the sample is similar to Canadian adults (15+) as established by the 2006 Canadian Census, with the exception that the present sample tends to include slightly fewer people aged 18 to 24, seniors 65 and older, single people, and has a somewhat higher level of educational attainment. Characteristics of the study sample are described in Table 1.

Table 1 Baseline sample characteristics

Measures

Severity of gambling problems was assessed by the Problem and Pathological Gambling Measure (PPGM) [19, 20]. The PPGM is a 14-item item instrument that provides both a continuous measure of problem gambling severity, as well as a categorical risk assessment that classifies people into the following categories: non-gambler; recreational gambler; at risk gambler; problem or pathological gambler. The PPGM has good internal consistency (Cronbach α =0.76 to 0.81, depending on the dataset) and 1 month test-re-test reliability (r = 0.78), as well as excellent correspondence to clinical assessment [20].

Religiosity was measured by the Rohrbaugh–Jessor Religiosity Scale (RJRS) [21]. This scale is an eight-item instrument assessing ritual, consequential, ideological, and experiential religiosity. Internal consistency of the scale was excellent (Cronbach α = .90). The first item of the scale, referring to past-year frequency of religious service attendance was examined separately in order to examine the community aspect of religious involvement, similar to previous research [6]. Response options for the measure of past-year religious service attendance were: 0 = Not at all; 1 = Once; 2 = Two to five times; 3 = Six to ten times; 4 = Once or twice a month; 5 = Once a week; 6 = More than once a week. Following Uecker and Stokes [6], responses were categorized as: ‘Not at all’, ‘Low’ = “Once” and “Two to five times”; ‘Moderate’ = “Six to ten times” and “Once or twice a month”; ‘High’ = “Once a week” and “More than once a week”.

The seven remaining items of the RJRS assessed the magnitude of religious belief in a person’s life, and each question had four or five response options indicating a range of agreement or importance. Specifically, five questions had five response options, and two questions had four response options. For example, response options for the following question: “When you have a serious personal problem, how often do you take religious advice or teaching into consideration?”, were: “almost always, usually, sometimes, and never” (scored 3, 2, 1, and 0, respectively). The responses from these seven items were summed together, with a range of scores falling between 0 and 26; higher scores indicated greater belief.

To assess one’s religious affiliation, respondents were asked ‘What is your religious affiliation?’ Response options were: Catholic, Protestant (e.g., Anglican, Baptist, Lutheran, United, Presbyterian), Atheist, Agnostic, Other (e.g., Muslim, Jewish, Buddhist, Hindu, Sikh), or Prefer not to say.

Statistical analyses

Mplus version 6.0 was used for all analyses. Following a stepwise-procedure [22], the first step was to fit a baseline or unconditional latent growth curve model to examine the overall trajectory of problem gambling severity. Next, the variables of interest were added to the model. To examine the potential protective effects of religiosity on problem gambling severity over time, two models were tested. The first model examined the frequency of past-year religious service attendance. The second model included an overall measure of religiosity. Both models included religious affiliation, and adjusted for age, education, household income, and marital status. This analytic approach employed a full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimator.

Latent growth curve models include two latent factors, the intercept and slope, as well as repeated measures of the observed outcome of interest over time. The slope factor loadings for the unconditional linear model were fixed at 0, 1, 2, 3, and 4, defining the start of the study as the intercept. To examine the potential moderating influence of gender, a multiple-group approach was used, whereby each level of the moderator was specified as a group (male and female). The multiple groups approach estimates growth curves based on the grouping of interest within a single analysis. Differential effects were examined by fixing regression co-efficients to be equal across groups (also known as equality constraints) and comparing the constrained model to the unconstrained model. If the chi-square statistic of the constrained model had increased significantly, the unconstrained model fit the data better than the constrained model, indicating a significant moderating effect of the grouping variable (gender in this case).

The following indices were used to examine model fit: comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). Adequate fit was indicated by CFI and TLI >0.90, RMSEA <0.08, and SRMR <0.10. Good fit was indicated by CFI and TLI >0.95, RMSEA <0.06, and SRMR <0.08 [23]. Important to note is that chi-square test of model fit is likely to be significant when the sample size is large, which is the case for the current study. Thus, model fit was based primarily on the CFI, TLI, RMSEA, and SRMR.

Results

Unconditional latent growth curve model

An overall decreasing trend that appeared linear was observed in mean scores based on the PPGM assessment across the five assessments (Table 2). The linear growth curve model fit the data well [χ2 (19) = 42.7, p = 0.001, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.03, SRMR = 0.02]. The mean latent variables representing the intercept and slope indicated similar baseline levels of problem gambling severity for males and females, as well as the same decreasing slope over time (Table 3). For both males and females, there was also significant variance in baseline levels of problem gambling severity, indicating that not everyone started at the same level of problem gambling severity. The significant variance in the slope indicated variability in individual trajectories over time. The significant negative correlations between the intercept and slope indicate that higher levels of problem gambling severity at baseline were associated with steeper declines over time. To test the moderating effect of gender, the constrained [χ2 (21) = 47.02] and unconstrained [χ2 (19) = 42.72] models were compared, and the results [χ2diff(2) = 4.3, p = 0.12] indicated no moderating effect of gender.

Table 2 Gambling severity categories and mean scores (SD) based on the Problem and Pathological Gambling Measure (PPGM) across the five data waves
Table 3 Unconditional latent growth curve model of problem gambling severity, by gender (n = 4120)

Conditional latent growth curve model: The influence of past-year frequency of religious service attendance

The conditional latent growth curve model estimating the effect of past-year religious service attendance showed good fit [χ2 (37) = 55.5, p = 0.03, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.02, SRMR = 0.01]. Among both males and females, those with high religious service attendance reported significantly lower levels of problem gambling severity at baseline (β = −0.62, p < 0.001, and β = −0.65, p < 0.001, respectively) compared to those who did not attend religious services at all in the past year. For females, even those with moderate past year attendance reported significantly lower levels of problem gambling severity at baseline (β = −0.16, p = 0.04) compared to those who did not attend at all. Furthermore, past-year religious service attendance influenced the slope of problem gambling severity for females, meaning that those with high past-year attendance, experienced a slower decline in problem gambling severity (β = 0.32, p = 0.03). In terms of the moderating effect of gender, a significant difference between the constrained and unconstrained model [χ2diff(13) = 192.9, p < 0.001] was observed indicating a moderating effect of gender on frequency of service attendance, such that the effect of past-year religious service attendance on problem gambling severity was greater among females.

The model with covariates also indicated good fit [χ2 (91) = 115.5, p = 0.04, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.01, SRMR = 0.01]. Table 4 presents the results from this model. All significant associations between past-year service attendance and problem gambling severity, as well as the moderating effect of gender [χ2diff(44) = 336.8, p < 0.001], were retained after including covariates in the model, with the exception of the influence of high past-year religious service attendance on the slope of problem gambling severity for females, which lost significance.

Table 4 The influence of frequency of religious service attendance on the intercept and slope of problem gambling severity, by gender (n = 3959)

Differences also emerged with respect to religious affiliation (Table 4 and Additional file 1: Table S1). Catholic males had significantly higher levels of problem gambling severity at baseline compared to Protestant males (β = 0.16, p = 0.02), and males who reported other religious affiliations (β = 0.32, p = 0.002). Catholic females had significantly higher levels of problem gambling severity at baseline compared to Protestant females (β = 0.12, p = 0.05), those who reported Other religious affiliations (β = 0.43, p < 0.001), as well as those who were Atheist/Agnostic (β = 0.30, p = 0.03). Protestant females had greater levels of problem gambling severity at baseline compared to those with Other religious affiliations (β =0.31, p = 0.001). In addition, males and females who preferred not to state their religious affiliation reported greater levels of problem gambling severity at baseline compared to those who reported other religious affiliations (β =0.31, p = 0.02 and β =0.29, p = 0.02). Religious affiliation did not influence the slope of problem gambling severity for males or females.

Conditional latent growth curve model: The influence of overall religiosity on problem gambling severity

The conditional latent growth curve model estimating the effect of past-year religiosity showed good fit [χ2 (25) = 47.7, p = 0.004, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.02, SRMR = 0.02]. For both males and females, overall religiosity was negatively associated with baseline levels of problem gambling severity, meaning that those with higher levels of religiosity reported lower levels of problem gambling severity at baseline (β = −0.17, p < 0.001, and β = −0.09, p < 0.001, respectively). The moderating effect of gender was significant [χ2diff(5) = 45.9, p < 0.001], indicating that the effect of religiosity on baseline levels of problem gambling severity was greater among males.

The model with covariates fit well [χ2 (79) = 113.9, p = 0.01, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.01, SRMR = 0.01]. Table 5 presents the results from this model. All significant associations between religiosity and problem gambling severity, as well as the moderating effect of gender [χ2diff(36) = 213.4, p < 0.001], were retained after including covariates in the model. Differences emerged with respect to religious affiliation (Table 5 and Additional file 2: Table S2). Catholic males had significantly higher levels of problem gambling severity at baseline compared to all other religious affiliations (Protestant: β = 0.14, p = 0.04; Atheist/Agnostic: β = 0.28, p = 0.01; Other: β = 0.33, p = 0.001), except for those who preferred to not state their religious affiliation. Protestant males reported greater levels of problem gambling severity compared to those with other religious affiliations (β = 0.19, p = 0.04). In addition, males and females who preferred not to state their religious affiliation, reported higher levels of problem gambling severity at baseline compared to those who reported other religious affiliations (β =0.28, p = 0.04 and β =0.33, p = 0.01, respectively).

Table 5 The influence of overall religiosity on the intercept and slope of problem gambling severity, by gender (n = 4120)

Catholic and Protestant females showed the same pattern of results. Both religious groups reported significantly higher levels of problem gambling severity at baseline compared to those who were atheist/agnostic, as well as those who reported other religious affiliations (Catholic females: β = 0.49, p < 0.001 and β = 0.44, p < 0.001, respectively; Protestant females: β = 0.41, p < 0.001 and β = 0.36, p < 0.001, respectively). In addition, females preferred not to state their religious affiliation reported greater levels of problem gambling severity at baseline compared to those who reported they were Atheist/Agnostic (β = 0.38, p = 0.005). Similar to overall religiosity, religious affiliation did not influence the slope of problem gambling severity either for males or females.

Discussion

The findings from the current study are consistent with previous longitudinal research demonstrating the transient nature of problem gambling [24]. In addition, there was a negative correlation between baseline levels of problem gambling severity and its rate of change: higher levels of problem gambling severity at baseline were associated with steeper declines over time. These findings are also similar to those from a recent study examining the latent growth trajectory of problem gambling severity using data from the Manitoba Longitudinal Study of Young Adults [25]. This naturally occurring, decreasing pattern has also been observed in trajectories of other behavioral addictions. For example, one study using data from the QLS examined trajectories of various normatively counter-indicated habits including excessive eating, sexual behavior, shopping, online chatting, eating, and video gaming, and found that the excessive behavior was highest at the initial assessment, followed by a decreasing trend [26]. Together, this set of findings suggests that problem gambling is episodic in nature, and at the sub-clinical level, may resolve naturally over time. Additionally, given that the QLS included non-clinical samples, it is possible that the survey itself may have acted as an intervention; cueing respondents to think about their behavior and to make changes over time.

The results of the present study are also in line with previous findings indicating that religious service attendance may be a more robust protective factor against gambling than other aspects of religiosity [27,28,29]. Specifically, we found that respondents who attended religious services weekly or more reported lower levels of problem gambling severity. Similarly, Uecker and Stokes [6] found that adolescents who attended religious services weekly or more had lower odds of having ever gambled. In terms of longitudinal associations in the current study, only weekly or more frequent past-year religious service attendance had a significant association with the slope of problem gambling severity. This association was significant only for females, and did not occur in the expected direction; those who attended religious services weekly or more experienced a slower decline in problem gambling severity over time. It is possible that the slower rate of decline was due to the fact that respondents who attended religious services weekly or more had lower levels of problem gambling severity to begin with. However, this association lost significance once covariates were added to the model, indicating that religious service attendance did not independently influence problem gambling severity over time. The moderating effect of gender indicated that the influence of past-year religious service attendance was greater among females; however, the effect of overall religiosity was greater among males. However, it is important to note that strong conclusions cannot be drawn without also examining the role of selection factors, such as personality factors, that might make it more likely that individuals attend religious services, and less likely to develop gambling problems. For example, conscientiousness has been found to be negatively associated with problem gambling [30]. Within the construct of conscientiousness, the facet of self-discipline may also be reflected in the measure of frequency of religious service attendance, that is, those that have greater self-discipline might attend religious services more often. Future research should seek to examine the potential explanatory role of personality traits, and in particular, the facet of self-discipline, in the association between religiosity and problem gambling.

In terms of religious affiliation, overall, our findings are in line with previous research. In the current study, Catholic males had greater levels of problem gambling severity compared to Protestant males. A finding that is in line with previous research indicating that Catholics gambled more frequently and were more likely to be problem gamblers compared to Protestants [10, 11, 31, 32]. Furthermore, those with no religious affiliation (atheist/agnostic), tended to have lower levels of problem gambling severity compared to either Protestant or Catholic groups. This is similar to previous research indicating that Catholics were more likely to have gambled compared to those with no religious affiliation [11, 33]. As a potential explanation for the somewhat counterintuitive finding that religious affiliation can be associated with a greater probability of engaging in risky behaviors, findings from a recent study indicated that if a certain behavior is not prohibited by the ethical code of a given religious affiliation (which in the case of gambling is largely variant across denominations), transcendental beliefs might increase perceived control and thus risk taking [34].

In addition, those who preferred not to state their religious affiliation reported greater levels of problem gambling severity at baseline, compared to those who reported ‘Other’ religious affiliations. This may be because those in the ‘Other’ group were predominantly from religious traditions that either explicitly denounce gambling or deem it as culturally unacceptable (i.e., Islam, Buddhism, and Sikhism). In addition, it is also possible that those who preferred not to report their religious affiliation had less trust in the world and society, or had experienced trauma due to religion, both of which can be significant sources of stress and anxiety. Since gambling can be a maladaptive way of coping with negative emotions, it is possible that these potential heightened levels of stress and anxiety may have triggered an increase in gambling among this group. However, this is merely speculation, and due to the nature of self-reported data, it is unknown which category of religious affiliation (if any), these respondents who preferred not to state their religious affiliation, might identify with, nor the specific reasons underlying the decision to not report one’s religious affiliation.

Also of note is the independent influence of different measures of religion—religious affiliation, religious service attendance, and overall religiosity—on problem gambling severity among adults. These findings highlight the multi-dimensional nature of religion, and the importance of examining a variety of religion measures to accurately capture this construct [10, 27].

Limitations

Given the low response rate (21.3%), the findings from the current study should be interpreted with caution. In addition, due to oversampling those at-risk, the QLS sample contained a somewhat higher proportion of At Risk, Problem, and Pathological Gamblers, and a lower proportion of Non-Gamblers compared to the province of Ontario, Canada (13.7% vs. 6.3%; 2.1% vs. 1.4%; 1.2% vs. 0.8%, respectively) [35]. However, according to the 2006 Canadian Census, the overall demographic profile of the QLS (i.e., gender, age, marital status, education, income, employment, and race/ethnicity) is similar to Canadian adults (15+), with some exceptions [16]. The QLS sample has a somewhat higher level of educational attainment, fewer people in the 18 to 24 and 65 years and older, age groups, as well as fewer single people, and visible minorities.

Although the religion measures used in the current study did influence the initial level of problem gambling severity, they did not influence the rate of change over time. One explanation for these null findings could be the use of time-invariant variables. It is likely that measures of religion are not static but instead change over time. Unfortunately, the QLS did not assess religiosity beyond the initial wave of data collection. Future research should seek to address this limitation using longitudinal datasets which capture measures of religiosity over time, and perhaps with the use of time-varying predictor variables and covariates, or alternatively with the use of a parallel-process growth curve model. Furthermore, the assumption in the current study, as well as all other studies conducted to date on this topic, is that religiosity predicts current and subsequent problem gambling severity. However, what has yet to be examined are potential causal effects in the opposite direction, and whether gambling predicts religiosity.

Another limitation revolves around the measure of religious affiliation. Specifically, with regards to the heterogeneity of the ‘Other’ category, which was comprised of diverse minority religions despite different perspectives on gambling. For example, within the Islamic tradition, gambling is explicitly prohibited in the Quran, whereas within the Jewish tradition, gambling is more culturally accepted and has a longstanding history. These diverse religions were grouped together because there was rather low variability in the population of Quinte overall; the ‘Other’ category represented only 9% of the total sample.

Conclusions

The findings from the current study suggest that the relationship between religiosity and problem gambling is complex and nuanced. For example, the findings indicate that some dimensions of religiosity, including religious service attendance and personal religiosity may play a protective role against problem gambling, while other aspects of religiosity such as one’s religious affiliation were associated with an increase in problem gambling severity, such as the case with Catholics compared to Atheists. Thus, there is a need for additional research to better understand the relationship between problem gambling and religiosity over time taking into consideration additional factors that might cause individuals to select into organized religion, including but not limited to personality factors such as conscientiousness. Overall, the findings from this study provide support for the potential influential role of religious affiliation and respective faith-based doctrines in shaping cultural views on gambling and ultimately behavior.

Abbreviations

CFI:

Comparative Fit Index

FIML:

Full Information Maximum Likelihood

PPGM:

Problem and Pathological Gambling Measure

QLS:

Quinte Longitudinal Study

RJRS:

Rohrbaugh-Jessor Religiosity Scale

RMSEA:

Root Mean Square Error of Approximation

SRMR:

Standardized Root Mean Square Residual

TLI:

Tucker-Lewis Index

References

  1. Koenig HG, King DE, Carson VB. Handbook of religion and health. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Koenig HG. Religion, spirituality, and health: the research and clinical implications. ISRN Psychiatry. 2012:1–33.

  3. Williams RJ, Royston J, Hagen BF. Gambling and problem gambling within forensic populations: a review of the literature. Crim Justice Behav. 2005;32(6):665–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Chou K-L, Afifi TO. Disordered (pathologic or problem) gambling and axis I psychiatric disorders: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on alcohol and related conditions. Am J Epidemiol. 2011;173(11):1289–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Petry NM, Armentano C. Prevalence, assessment, and treatment of pathological gambling: a review. Psychiatr Serv. 1999;50(8):1021–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Uecker JE, Stokes CE. Religious background and gambling among young adults in the United States. J Gambl Stud. 2016;32(1):341–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Ghandour LA, El Sayed DS. Gambling behaviors among university youth: does one’s religious affiliation and level of religiosity play a role? Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(1):279–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Casey DM, Williams RJ, Mossière AM, Schopflocher DP, el-Guebaly N, Hodgins DC, et al. The role of family, religiosity, and behavior in adolescent gambling. J Adolesc. 2011;34(5):841–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Hodgins DC, Schopflocher DP, Martin CR, el-Guebaly N, Casey DM, Currie SR, et al. Disordered gambling among higher-frequency gamblers: who is at risk? Psychol Med. 2012;42(11):2433–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Ellison CG, Mcfarland MJ. Religion and gambling among U.S. adults: exploring the role of traditions, beliefs, practices, and networks. J Sci Study Relig. 2011;50(1):82–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Diaz JD. Religion and gambling in sin-city: a statistical analysis of the relationship between religion and gambling patterns in Las Vegas residents. Soc Sci J. 2000;37(3):453–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Fattore L, Altea S, Fratta W. Sex differences in drug addiction: a review of animal and human studies. Women’s Health. 2008;4(1):51–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. MacLaren VV, Best LA. Multiple addictive behaviors in young adults: student norms for the shorter PROMIS questionnaire. Addict Behav. 2010;35(3):252–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Miller AS, Hoffmann JP. Risk and religion: an explanation of gender differences in religiosity. J Sci Study Relig. 1995;34(1):63–75.

  15. Miller AS, Stark R. Gender and religiousness: can socialization explanations be saved? Am J Sociol. 2002;107(6):1399–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Williams RJ, Hann RG, Schopflocher DP, West BL, McLaughlin P, White N, et al. Quinte longitudinal study of gambling and problem gambling. Report prepared for the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre. Guelph, Ontario, Canada. 2015. Available from: http://hdl.handle.net/10133/3641. Accessed May 2017.

  17. Konkolÿ Thege B, Colman I, el-guebaly N, Hodgins DC, Patten SB, Schopflocher D, et al. Substance-related and behavioural addiction problems: two surveys of Canadian adults. Addict Res Theory. 2015;23(1):34–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. McLaughlin P, White N, King K, Hann RG, Williams RJ, Schopflocher D, West B, Flexhaug T. QLS front-line retention manual: methods for achieving a 94% cohort retention rate in longitudinal research. Report prepared for the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre. Guelph, Ontario, Canada. 2014. Available from: http://hdl.handle.net/10133/3379. Accessed May 2017.

  19. Williams RJ, Volberg R. Best Practices in the Population Assessment of Problem Gambling. Report prepared for the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre. Guelph, Ontario, Canada. 2010. Available from: http://hdl.handle.net/10133/1259. Accessed May 2017.

  20. Williams RJ, Volberg RA. The classification accuracy of four problem gambling assessment instruments in population research. Int Gambl Stud. 2014;14(1):15–28.

  21. Rohrbaugh J, Jessor R. Religiosity in youth: a personal control against deviant behavior. J Pers. 1975;43(157):136–55.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Bollen KA, Curran PJ. Latent curve models: a structural equation perspective. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6(1):1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. LaPlante DA, Nelson SE, LaBrie RA, Shaffer HJ. Stability and progression of disordered gambling: lessons from longitudinal studies. Can J Psychiatr. 2008;53(1):52–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Edgerton JD, Melnyk TS, Roberts LW. Problem gambling and the youth-to-adulthood transition: assessing problem gambling severity trajectories in a sample of young adults. J Gambl Stud. 2015;31(4):1463–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Konkolÿ Thege B, Woodin EM, Hodgins DC, Williams RJ. Natural course of behavioral addictions: a 5-year longitudinal study. BMC Psychiatry. 2015;15(1):4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Beyerlein K, Sallaz JJ. Faith’s wager: how religion deters gambling. Soc Sci Res. 2017;62:204–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hoffmann JP. Religion and problem gambling in the U.S. Rev Relig Res. 2000;41(4):488–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Lam D. The influence of religiosity on gambling participation. J Gambl Stud. 2006;22(3):305–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Hwang JY, Shin YC, Lim SW, Park HY, Shin NY, Jang JH, et al. Multidimensional comparison of personality characteristics of the big five model, impulsiveness, and affect in pathological gambling and obsessive–compulsive disorder. J Gambl Stud. 2012;28(3):351–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Hraba J, Lee G. Gender, gambling and problem gambling. J Gambl Stud. 1996;12(1):83–101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Feigelman W, Wallisch LS, Lesieur HR. Problem gamblers, problem substance users, and dual-problem individuals: an epidemiological study. Am J Public Health. 1998;88(3):467–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Eitle D. Religion and gambling among young adults in the United States. J Sci Study Relig. 2011;50(1):61–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Chan KQ, Tong EMW, Tan YL. Taking a leap of faith. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2014;5(8):901–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Williams RJ, Volberg RA. Gambling and problem gambling in Ontario. Report prepared for the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre and the Ontario Ministry of Health and long term care. Available from: https://www.uleth.ca/dspace/handle/10133/3378. Accessed May 2017.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

The Quinte Longitudinal Study (QLS) was funded by the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre (OPGRC). The funding body had no involvement in the study design, analysis, interpretation of the data, in the writing of the report, and in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Availability of data and materials

Please contact Gambling Research Exchange Ontario (GREO) for access to data on which the manuscript is based.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RJW designed and managed the Quinte Longitudinal Study. BKT, RJW, and DCH conceptualized the hypotheses for this paper. SMP designed the analytic approach and conducted the statistical analyses. SMP, BKT, DCH, and RJW interpreted the results. SMP prepared the first draft of the manuscript. All authors read, contributed to, and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Seema Mutti-Packer.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval was provided by the Human Subject Research Committee at the University of Lethbridge. If the person agreed to participate in the survey, they were sent an email with a link to the online questionnaire or booked into a time slot at the program office where they completed the survey on a computer on site. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants prior to completing the online survey, each year.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Contrasts between all categories of religious affiliation when examining the influence of frequency of religious service attendance on the intercept and slope of problem gambling severity, by gender (n = 3959). Table presenting all contrasts between religious affiliation. (DOCX 14 kb)

Additional file 2: Table S2.

Contrasts between all categories of religious affiliation when examining the influence of overall religiosity on the intercept and slope of problem gambling severity, by gender (n = 4120). Table presenting all contrasts between religious affiliation. (DOCX 14 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mutti-Packer, S., Hodgins, D.C., Williams, R.J. et al. The protective role of religiosity against problem gambling: findings from a five-year prospective study. BMC Psychiatry 17, 356 (2017). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12888-017-1518-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12888-017-1518-5

Keywords