Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Interactions between risky decisions, impulsiveness and smoking in young tattooed women

Abstract

Background

According to previous studies, one of the common problems of everyday life of persons with tattoos is risky behavior. However, direct examination of the decision making process, as well as factors which determine women’s risk-taking decisions to get tattoos, have not been conducted. This study investigates whether risk taking decision-making is associated with the self-assessment impulsiveness in tattooed women.

Methods

Young women (aged 18–35 years) with (N = 60) and without (N = 60) tattoos, performed the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), as a measure of decision-making processes, as well as completing the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS-11).

Results

Tattooed women showed significantly higher scores in the BIS-11 and preference for disadvantageous decks on the IGT compared to non-tattooed women. There was no significant correlation between risky decision-making in the IGT and BIS-11 impulsivity measures. A significantly higher rate of smoking was observed in the tattooed women. However, the analysis did not reveal a group effect after adjustment for smoking in the IGT and the BIS-11 measures.

Conclusions

The present study was specifically designed to resolve questions regarding associations between impulsiveness and risky decision-making in tattooed women. It shows that in tattooed women, risky decisions are not a direct result of their self-reported impulsiveness. Smoking does not explain the psychometric differences between tattooed women and controls.

Peer Review reports

Background

Tattooing is s phenomenon becoming increasingly common among individuals [1]. Although tattooing is often suggested to be a masculine trait [2, 3] it has been reported that women make up to 45-65% of the tattooed population [4, 5]. Though the popularity of body modification increases, psychosocial data about tattooing behavior are few and controversial [6, 7].

Why do some women get tattoos despite possible negative consequences?

It was suggested by some physicians that finding a tattoo during physical examination should alert to the possibility of an underlying wide range of psychopathological conditions [810]. Tattoos are associated with personality characteristics [11] such “sensation seeking” and impulsiveness [1215] and with cluster B personality disorders [16, 17]. Tattooed subjects rated themselves as more adventurous, creative, artistic, individualistic, attractive and risk-takers than those without tattoos [15, 18, 19]. Participants who experienced sexual abuse often stated that they use tattoos to overcome certain traumatic experiences, and those with high numbers of tattoos were characterized by an addiction-like drive to continue body modification [6].

Tattooing was also found to be associated with a wide range of impulsivity-related behaviors such as: violence, weekly alcohol consumption, illicit drug use, dropping out of school, greater numbers of lifetime sexual partners, unprotected sex, suicidal attempts, death by homicide [2028] and shoplifting [15]. According to previous studies, one of the common problems of everyday life of persons with tattoos is risk-taking behavior [12, 29, 30] leading to medical complications (e.g., potential diseases, allergies, or infections after tattooing; [31], especially the transmission of hepatitis C [32]. High tattoo prevalence was found among unemployed young women who do not live in a stable partnership [33].

However, direct examinations of the decision-making process, as well as factors that determine the risk-taking decision to get tattoos, have not been conducted amongst women.

Decision-making is a cognitive function concerned with the process of reflecting on the consequences of a certain choice [34]. Despite evidence of a range of behavioral deficits as the precursors of getting tattooed, the basis for the risk-taking behavior has not been completely characterized. The basis of decision strategy in tattooed subjects was not previously investigated.

Most existing theories suggest impulsiveness as the main reason for getting a tattoo. It seems plausible to assume that impaired decision making reflects a variety of impulse control problems [35]. However, the association between the decision-making process and impulsivity is unclear [36, 37] and their role in tattooed populations has not been investigated.

The Iowa Gambling Test (IGT) is designed as a measure of risky decision making [38]. This task is the most popular measure of decision-making processes (for a review, see [39]) that mimics real-life decisions [40]. In IGT, each choice is ambiguous, at least initially, with regard to the outcome. Effective performance on the IGT depends on the ability to learn to avoid risky card decks and instead develop a preference for safe ones [41, 42].

Risky decision-making may be a predictor of tattooing behavior, because both result from interacting impulsiveness traits. Some authors found an association between self-reported impulsiveness and decision making performance [36, 4346]. However, no such association was found by others between impulsivity and risky decision-making [4753]. If indeed impulsivity and risky decision-making decompose into two independent constructs - then each can predict tattooing behavior.

Previous studies showed that tattooing behavior was associated with higher smoking rates as compared to the general population [15, 54]. A large body of research has examined the association of cigarette use with individual differences in the decision making processes and impulsivity. A review of this literature suggests that both risky decision-making [55] and impulsiveness [56] constitute important correlates of tobacco use. Tobacco use can be a significant confounder that mediates among risky decision-making, impulsiveness and tattooing behavior. Such association between tattooing behavior and smoking may be largely related to non-rational decision making and non-planning behavior. The present study aimed to evaluate the association of risky decision-making and impulsiveness with tobacco use in tattooed women. We hypothesized that measures of risky decisions and impulsiveness would be associated with tattooing behavior, but that these associations are independent of tobacco use.

The major issues of the present study were: (1) Do personality trait such as impulsiveness and risky decision-making interact in predicting tattooing behavior? (2) Which of them is the best predictor for tattooing behavior?

To this end, we compared the level of impulsiveness as measured by the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS-11) and by the IGT, in healthy tattooed women with that of healthy untattooed women. We predicted that: (1) women with tattoos would report higher impulsiveness scores on the BIS-11 than women without tattoos; (2) women with tattoos would perform worse than women without tattoos on the IGT; (3) there would be a significant correlation between the BIS-11score and the IGT performance among women with tattoos, and (4) Motor, non-planning and attentional impulsiveness factors of the BIS-11 as expressions of specific facets of impulsivity would be related to specific neuropsychological mechanisms of risky decision-making in young tattooed women (5) smoking status, however, would not explain these associations in the tattooed women.

Methods

All participants (tattooed women and control women) were recruited by means of advertisements posted at universities, through personal contacts and via social networks (Facebook), to take part in a research project investigating decision making styles between tattooed women and those without tattoos. All participants were recruited throughout the Tel Aviv area between March 2012 and July 2012. The participants in both groups (research and control) were either employed or students or graduates and belonged to similar socioeconomic backgrounds. The participation in the study was voluntary and without payment. As compensation for participating in the study participants received free charge consultation regarding their decision-making style as well as professional advice regarding the results of the neuro-cognitive and personality assessments. Individual sessions were conducted for the purpose of the study. Participants were given an explanation regarding the research aims and signed a consent form indicating their willingness to participate in the research which included the computerized neuropsychological examination, the personality tests, and a special questionnaire covering extensive background information. The duration of each individual session was up to an hour and a half, with the entire research process taking place over a period of five months.

Sixty women volunteers with tattoos aged 18 to 35 years (M = 28.4, SD = 5.95) were included in the study. Fifty eight percents of the tattooed women had more than one tattoo. All the participants were tattooed at the time of the study (women with removed tattoos were not included). The participants were either employed or students with the following education level: high school diploma or lower – 46.7%, undergraduate university degree – 25%, Master’s university degree– 23.3%, or Doctoral degree – 5%.

We analyzed only tattooed women in order to avoid gender differences on the IGT performance [57]. We expected that comorbid neurological problems, alcohol use disorders and drug dependence would result in an additive effect on neuro-cognitive deficiencies [53]. Thus, the exclusion criteria were neurological disorders, mental retardation, alcohol and substance abuse/dependence (other than tobacco smoking), major psychiatric disorders and treatment with any psychiatric medication. It was established that 55% of participants from the tattooed group were smokers. A semi-structured interview of 20-items to measure the tattooed women’s characteristics was administered by one of the researchers (AK).

The control group included 60 healthy volunteers in a similar age range as the study group, namely 18–35 years old (M = 28.5, SD = 5.43) and recruited from the same area. Education level in the control group was as follows: high school diploma or lower – 25%, undergraduate university degree – 28.3%, Master’s degree – 41.7% and Doctoral degree – 5%. All participants completed a screening interview, which covered the following areas: medical history, illicit drug use, family and personal psychiatric history. None of the subjects received any psychopharmacological treatment. Exclusion criteria for the untattooed control women (C) included any current or past DSM-IV-TR axis I psychiatric disorder. Only 10% of participants from this group smoked regularly.

The study was approved by the Bar-Ilan University Ethics Review Board (Ramat Gan, Israel).

Decision making measures: computerized animated variant of the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)

We applied a modified computerized animated time-unlimited version of the IGT [58] - Casino task (AnimaScan Ltd, Ashdod, Israel, 2000), for details see [52]. Briefly, the IGT requires individuals to select cards from four different decks, called A, B, C, and D. Two of the decks, A and B, often result in high gains ($100) but also carry a high risk of large losses thus leading to a cumulative long-term loss. Therefore, these decks are disadvantageous (“bad”). The remaining two decks (labeled C & D) typically result in lower rewards ($50), but also generate lower losses, resulting in a cumulative long-term gain and are therefore advantageous (“better”).

Prior to starting the IGT, the participants were told that the goal of the game was to win as much money as possible and avoid losses to the best of their abilities. They were told that they may choose cards from any deck, and that they may switch decks at any time. Participants were also informed that some of the decks are more advantageous than others, and in order to win, one must avoid the “bad” decks and stick to the “better” ones [38], but they had to find out by themselves which decks were “bad” and which “better”.

Each participant chose 100 cards which were then analyzed by dividing them into 5 blocks of 20 cards each. We calculated a net score for each block by subtracting the number of advantageous cards from the number of disadvantageous ones [(C + D) - (A + B)] for each of the 20 card blocks. A score below 0 signified that subjects adopted an overall disadvantageous strategy (more card selections in decks A and B), while a score above 0 implied a more advantageous deck preference (more card selections in decks C and D).

Barratt impulsiveness scale (BIS-11)

The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) is a 30 item self-report instrument designed to assess the personality/behavioral construct of impulsiveness [59]. Participants rate the 30 statements on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1–4: 1 = never/rarely, 2 = sometimes, 3 = frequently and 4 = almost always/always. The higher the total score, the higher the self-reported level of impulsivity (total scores range from 30 to 120). The BIS-11 is the most commonly administered self-report measure for the assessment of impulsiveness in both research and clinical settings [60].

Factor analysis of the BIS-11 reveals three subscales: motor impulsiveness, which reflects action without forethought (i.e. I do things without thinking); non-planning impulsiveness, reflecting the focus being on the present (i.e. I am more interested in the present than the future); and attentional impulsiveness that reflects reduced ability to maintain attention on a stimulus (i.e., I do not 'pay attention’; See [59] for a list of items comprising BIS-11 subscales).

Statistical procedure

The data was analyzed using the SAS v9.1 statistical software package for Windows (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). The first step examined the difference between groups on socio-demographic variables (age, education and smoking habit). T-test was used to analyze numerical variables (age and education) and a chi-square (χ 2) test for the categorical variable (smoking habit). In the second step Pearson’s correlation test was used to assess the relationships between the demographic characteristics, self-report measures of the BIS-11 and the IGT performance.

In the third step MANCOVA was used to examine the influence of the group (tattooed or control) on common self-report measures of impulsiveness (BIS-11 subscales) while controlling for socio-demographic characteristics. To analyze the effect of group differences in the decision making process (the five blocks of the IGT), repeated measures ANCOVA was performed with socio-demographic characteristics as covariates. Since ANCOVA assumes that a dependent variable is linearly related to the covariates, correlation analysis was performed to evaluate the relationship between socio-demographic characteristics and behavioral measurements.

Results

Between-group comparison of the socio-demographic characteristics

Univariate analysis did not show differences between groups with regard to age (t = 0.11, df = 118, p = 0.91), but significant differences were found regarding education (t = 2.60, df = 118, p = 0.01). Women with tattoos were less educated. A significant difference was also found on smoking habit (χ 2 = 27.69, df = 1, p < 0.0001). The proportion of smokers in the tattooed group was 5 time higher than in the control group (Table 1).

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of demographic characteristics, measures of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale -11 and the Iowa Gambling Task performance in women with and without tattoos

Because education and smoking habit were found to be significantly different between groups they were considered as potential covariates. MANCOVA analysis assumes that the dependent variable should be linearly related to the covariate (education). Correlation analysis showed no linear relationship between impulsivity measures and education (Table 2). Since there was also no relationship between level of education and IGT scores, education was excluded from the list of potential covariates.

Table 2 Correlations between demographic characteristics, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) measures and Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) performance

Between-group comparison of the IGT performance

A repeated measure ANCOVA was used with the groups (tattooed vs. controls) as between-subjects factor, block (each 20 trials) as a within-subjects factor; smoking status as covariate and the net score of IGT as the dependent variable. The results showed an effect of block and an effect of “group x block” interaction: F(4,116) = 3.49, p = 0.0103, indicating that task performance increased consecutively from block to block; F(4,116) = 3.64, p = 0.0081, and that the women with tattoos performed worse than controls (see Figure 1) on the second, fourth and fifth block of IGT (Table 3). The control group improved significantly their learning curve. Such an improvement was not obtained in the tattooed women. There was no group effect after adjustment for smoking habit F(1,119) = 3.87, p = 0.051.

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean Iowa Gambling Task net scores on each of the five blocks. The performance of the tattooed and non-tattooed groups. Performance in IGT block 1, block 2 and block 3 did not differ between the two groups, but the differences between group differences became significant in blocks 4 and 5. The positive net score of the non-tattooed group from block 2 to 5 can be explained by their fast learning in contrast to the tattooed women, who did not improve during the task and exhibited non-optimal outcomes because they failed to correct disadvantageous choices.

Table 3 Interaction between each group and Iowa Gambling Test’s blocks: pairwise comparisons of performance measures

Between-group comparison of the barratt BIS-11 measures

A one-way (group) MANCOVA revealed significant multivariate group effect (Wilk’s Lambda = 0.82, F(3,114) = 8.57, p < 0.0001) in all BIS-11 subscales: motor impulsiveness, non-planning impulsiveness, and attentional impulsiveness scores (Table 4), indicating that the groups differed significantly on the combined set of BIS-11 subscales.

Table 4 Analysis of Covariance (MANCOVA) on impulsivity measures

No overall smoking effect was found on BIS measures as shown by Wilk’s Lambda = 0.93, F(3,114) = 2.68, p = 0.05. Neither was an overall “smoking x group” interaction effect found, as shown by Wilk’s Lambda = 0.97, F(3,114) = 1.3, p = 0.28. As expected, examination of the BIS-11 subscales separately, using univariate test, showed that women with tattoos self-rated themselves significantly higher on all impulsivity domains than controls (Table 4). Smoking habit was significant only for Motor Impulsivity (F = 7.90, p = 0.0058). Interaction between smoking habit and group was not significant on all measures. The adjusted means of impulsivity measures were slightly larger than unadjusted means (Table 4).

Association between risky decision-making and impulsiveness measures

Table 2 shows absence of association between self-assessed impulsivity measures on the BIS-11 scales and risky decision-making on the IGT, indicating that these two measures assess different aspects of risk taking behavior.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to determine if measures of impulsivity (the BIS-11) and risky decision-making (the IGT) are associated with tattoo-related behavior in a sample of young healthy women. Our hypothesis was that both self-reported impulsiveness (BIS-11) and risky decisions in IGT, can predict independently the tattoo-related behavior. Our results indicate that both impulsivity (as measured by a questionnaire) and risk-taking decisions (as measured by a behavioral task) predicted presence of tattoos.

Risky decision-making among tattooed women

To the best of our knowledge this is the first experimental study of the decision making process in tattooed women. It was found that risky decision-making was associated with the selection of big gains albeit with maximal losses (the disadvantageous decks). In contrast, non-risky decision-making was associated with shifting selections to smaller gain but minimizing losses (the advantageous decks). The development of successful decision-making strategies after multiple evaluations of winning and losing are associated with higher net scores [58].

In the initial phase of the IGT the subjects make choices in conditions of maximal uncertainty. The decisions in the “ambiguity” phase were most likely made without awareness to the probabilities of reward or loss [61]. Decisions in the initial phase of the IGT did not differ between tattooed women and controls. This result is expected since random responses are a common strategy in uncertain situations [62].

The second part of the IGT performance constitutes “decision-making under risk” [47], in which subjects become more knowledgeable on the risks associated with each deck. The performance of the tattooed women differed significantly only at this stage: controls gradually shifted to advantageous decisions while the tattooed women continued to make disadvantageous decision as the task progressed (see Figure 1). Risky decision-making by tattooed women may result from their impaired ability to learn from the association between the actions and subsequent negative outcomes.

Association between impulsivity and risky decision-making among tattooed women

The main hypothesis of the study was that tattooed women make risky decisions as a result of their impulsiveness. Trait impulsivity, as measured by BIS-11, is associated with learning impairment in problem solving situations [63]. The BIS-11 measures the tendency of individuals to consider negative consequences before acting [64]. Buelow and Suhr [39] found that riskier IGT performance is related to higher levels of sensation seeking and impulsivity. Risky performance in the IGT may result from a personality trait that causes participants to minimize their consideration of future consequences, either positive or negative [65]. Thus, the high self-reported impulsiveness on the BIS-11 scale is expected to reflect the risky decision-making on the IGT. The use of multifactorial impulsivity scales enables detection of specific facets of impulsivity, related to risky decision making (Table 2). This is the first study that uses a neurocognitive measurement of risky decision making and shows that, in contrast to our hypothesis, risky decision making in tattooed women is not a result of self-reported impulsiveness. According to Bechara [66] impulsiveness is fundamentally different from risky decision-making. The latter involves choosing wrongly when presented with several alternatives, while impulsivity represents inhibition dysregulation. A decision process requires weighing the pros and cons of various choices against each other and acting based on the results of this comparison. Although there is some overlap between trait impulsiveness and risky decision-making [67], it appears that they may represent separate independent entities. Our findings are consistent with previous studies indicating that the dimensions of impulsiveness are distinct and uncorrelated to risky decision-making [4753].

Association between tattoos and smoking in women

Similar to previous studies, we found a significant association between tattooing and smoking [15, 54]. High impulsivity and risky decision-making in the tattoo group may be related to smoking as a confounding factor. Previous studies clearly implicate impulsivity as a precursor for smoking [see [6871]]. In addition, association between tattooing and smoking may be related to the fact that both may have addictive characteristics [see for review: [72]]. Non-smoking tattooed women were more impulsive than non-smoking controls, as assessed by the BIS-11. In contrast, in smoking participants the between-group differences in the BIS-11 scores were lost. Thus it seems that impulsivity measures in tattooed women are smoking-independent. Namely, the smoking status is not a direct mediator of tattooing behavior.

Limitations

Certain limitations impact the interpretation of our results. First, we did not examine comorbid Axis II disorders in the tattooed women, which may account for the neuropsychological findings. Future studies on this topic should assess personality psychopathology using appropriate structured clinical interviews. Second, this study was conducted among women with relatively small numbers of tattoos and without piercings who may be less impulsive than heavily tattooed or body pierced women [73, 74]. Third, participants with alcohol and drug use comorbidity were excluded. The presence of both tattoos and substance use disorders may have exerted additive effects on the IGT performance in the disadvantageous direction.

Conclusion

The present study was specifically designed to resolve questions regarding associations between impulsiveness and risky decision-making in tattooed women. It found that in this case, risky decisions are not a direct result of their self-reported impulsiveness.

Women with tattoos show substantial differences in decision-making from women without tattoos. Women with tattoos exhibited higher scores in the BIS-11, and failed in decision making tasks such as the IGT. But their impairments do not provide definitive evidence against a close association between impulsiveness and decision making. The association between impulsiveness and tattooing behavior was found to be independent of smoking status. There are a variety of environmental, social and psychological variables that lead to tattooing in young people. Our study highlights that impulsiveness and risky decision-making may be key factors in identifying individuals who are at risk for tattooing behavior. Identifying contributing factors could limit risky decisions associated with tattooing. The complex relationship among tattooing, impulsiveness, risky decision-making, Axis I and II psychopathology, gender, substance use disorders, sociodemographic charachteristics and particular brain mechanisms underlying the impaired decision making process merits large-scale investigation.

References

  1. Tiggemann M, Golder F: Tattooing: an expression of uniqueness in the appearance domain. Body Image. 2006, 3 (4): 309-315. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.09.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Armstrong M, Owen D, Roberts A, Koch J: College tattooing: more than skin deep. Dermatol Nurs. 2002, 14 (5): 317-323.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Armstrong M, Murphy K, Sallee A, Watson M: Tattooed army soldiers: examining the incidence, behavior, and risk. Mil Med. 2000, 165 (2): 135-141.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Armstrong M: Career-oriented women with tattoos image. J Nurs Scholarship. 1991, 23 (4): 215-220. 10.1111/j.1547-5069.1991.tb00674.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Laumann A, Derrick A: Tattoos and body piercings in the United States: a national data set. J Am Acad Dermatol. 2006, 55 (3): 413-421. 10.1016/j.jaad.2006.03.026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Stirn A, Oddo S, Peregrinova L, Philipp S, Hinz A: Motivations for body piercings and tattoos the role of sexual abuse and the frequency of body modifications. Psychiatry Res. 2011, 190 (2–3): 359-363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Latreille J, Levy J, Guinot C: Decorative tattoos and reasons for their removal: a prospective study in 151 adults living in South of France. J Eur Acad Dermatol Venereol. 2011, 25 (2): 181-187. 10.1111/j.1468-3083.2010.03738.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Cossio ML, Giesen LF, Araya G, Perez-Cotapos ML: Association between tattoos, piercings and risk behaviors in adolescents. Rev Med Chil. 2012, 140 (2): 198-206. 10.4067/S0034-98872012000200008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Manuel L, Retzlaff PD: Psychopathology and tattooing among prisoners. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2002, 46 (5): 522-531. 10.1177/030662402236738.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Borokhov A, Bastiaans R, Lerner V: Tattoo designs among drug abusers. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci. 2006, 43 (1): 28-33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Degelman D, Price ND: Tattoos and ratings of personal characteristics. Psychol Rep. 2002, 90 (2): 507-514. 10.2466/pr0.2002.90.2.507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Armstrong M, Pace-Murphy K: Tattooing: another risk-behavior in adolescents warranting national health teaching. Appl Nurs Res. 1997, 10 (4): 181-189. 10.1016/S0897-1897(97)80560-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Farrow J, Schwartz R, Vanderleeuw J: Tattooing behavior in adolescence. Am J Dis Child. 1991, 145 (2): 184-187. 10.1001/archpedi.1991.02160020076021.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Irwin C, Millstein S: Biopsychosocial correlates of risk-taking behaviors during adolescence. J Adolesc Health Care. 1986, 7: 82-93.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Drews DR, Allison CK, Probst JR: Behavioral and self-concept differences in tattooed and nontattooed college students. Psychol Rep. 2000, 86 (2): 475-481. 10.2466/pr0.2000.86.2.475.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Raspa RF, Cusack J: Psychiatric implications of tattoos. Am Fam Physician. 1990, 41 (5): 1481-1486.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Manuel L, Retzlaff P: Psychopathology and tattooing among prisoners. Int J Offender Ther. 2002, 46 (5): 522-531. 10.1177/030662402236738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Antoszewski B, Sitek A, Fijałkowska M, Kasielska A, Kruk-Jeromin J: Tattooing and body piercing–what motivates you to do it?. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2010, 56 (5): 471-479. 10.1177/0020764009106253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Swami V: Marked for life? A prospective study of tattoos on appearance anxiety and dissatisfaction, perceptions of uniqueness, and self-esteem. Body Image. 2011, 8 (3): 237-244. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.04.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Blackburn J, Cleveland J, Griffin R, Davis GG, Lienert J, McGwin G: Tattoo frequency and types among homicides and other deaths, 2007–2008: a matched case–control study. Am J Forensic Med Pathol. 2012, 33 (3): 202-205. 10.1097/PAF.0b013e318221b67d.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Braithwaite R, Robillard A, Woodring T, Stephens T, Arriola KJ: Tattooing and body piercing among adolescent detainees: relationship to alcohol and other drug use. J Subst Abuse. 2001, 13 (1–2): 5-16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Carroll L, Anderson R: Body piercing, tattooing, self-esteem, and body investment in adolescent girls. Adolescence. 2002, 37 (147): 627-637.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Forbes G: College students with tattoos and piercings: motives, family experiences, personality factors, and perception by others. Psychol Rep. 2001, 89 (3): 774-786. 10.2466/pr0.2001.89.3.774.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Heywood W, Patrick K, Smith AM, Simpson JM, Pitts MK, Richters J, Shelley JM: Who gets tattoos? Demographic and behavioral correlates of ever being tattooed in a representative sample of men and women. Ann Epidemiol. 2012, 22 (1): 51-56. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2011.10.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kuczkowski K: Diagnostic tattoo in a parturient with «ecstasy» use. Anaesthesia. 2003, 58 (12): 1251-1252.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Stephens M: Behavioral risks associated with tattooing. Fam Med. 2003, 35 (1): 52-54.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Pozgain I, Barkic J, Filakovic P: Tattoo and personality traits in croatian veterans. Yonsei Med J. 2004, 45 (2): 300-305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Yen CF, Hsiao RC, Yen JY, Yeh YC, Wang PW, Lin HC, Ko CH: Tattooing among high school students in southern Taiwan: the prevalence, correlates and associations with risk-taking behaviors and depression. Kaohsiung J Med Sci. 2012, 28 (7): 383-389. 10.1016/j.kjms.2011.10.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Anderson R: Tattooing should be regulated. New Engl J Med. 1992, 326 (3): 207-

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Sсhiffmacher H: 1000 Tatoos. 1996, Taschen

    Google Scholar 

  31. Long G, Rickman L: Infectious complications of tattoos. Clin Infect Dis. 1994, 18 (4): 610-619. 10.1093/clinids/18.4.610.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Nishioka S de A, Gyorkos TW: Tattoos as risk factors for transfusion-transmitted diseases. Int J Infect Dis. 2001, 5 (1): 27-34. 10.1016/S1201-9712(01)90045-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Stirn A: “My body belongs to me”- cultural history and psychology of piercings and tattoos. Ther Umsch. 2007, 64 (2): 115-119. 10.1024/0040-5930.64.2.115.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Bechara A: Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci. 2005, 8 (11): 1458-1463. 10.1038/nn1584.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Morgan M, Impallomeni L, Pirona A, Rogers R: Elevated impulsivity and impaired decision-making in abstinent ecstasy (MDMA) users compared to polydrug and drug-naive controls. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006, 31 (7): 1562-1573. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300953.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Franken I, Strien J, Muris P: Impulsivity is associated with behavioral decision- making deficits. Psychiat Res. 2008, 158 (2): 155-163. 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Upton DJ, Bishara AJ, Ahn W-Y, Stout JC: Propensity for risk taking and trait impulsivity in the Iowa Gambling Task. Pers Individ Dif. 2011, 50 (4): 492-495. 10.1016/j.paid.2010.11.013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Bechara A: Iowa gambling task professional manual. 2007, Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources

    Google Scholar 

  39. Buelow MT, Suhr JA: Construct validity of the Iowa Gambling Task. Neuropsychol Rev. 2009, 19 (1): 102-114. 10.1007/s11065-009-9083-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Bowman CH, Turnbull OH: Real versus facsimile reinforcers on the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain Cognition. 2003, 53 (2): 207-210. 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00111-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Dunn BD, Dalgleish T, Lawrence AD: The somatic marker hypothesis: a critical evaluation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006, 30 (2): 239-271. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Fellows LK: The cognitive neuroscience of human decision making: a review and conceptual framework. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2004, 3 (3): 159-172. 10.1177/1534582304273251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Crone E, Vendel I, Van der Molen M: Decision making in disinhibited adolescents and adults: insensitivity to future consequences or driven by immediate reward?. Pers Indiv Differ. 2003, 35 (7): 1625-1641. 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00386-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Suhr J, Tsanadisa J: Affect and personality correlates of the Iowa Gambling Task. Pers Indiv Differ. 2007, 43 (1): 27-36. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Sweitzer MM, Allen PA, Kaut KP: Relation of individual differences in impulsivity to nonclinical emotional decision making. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2008, 14 (5): 878-882.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Zermatten A, Van der Linden M, d’Acremont M, Jermann F, Bechara A: Impulsivity and decision making. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2005, 193 (10): 647-650. 10.1097/01.nmd.0000180777.41295.65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Brand M, Recknor EC, Grabenhorst F, Bechara A: Decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: Correlations with executive functions and comparisons of two different gambling tasks with implicit and explicit rules. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc. 2007, 29 (1): 86-99. 10.1080/13803390500507196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Bazanis E, Rogers RD, Dowson JH, Taylor P, Meux C, Staley C, Nevinson-Andrews D, Taylor C, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ: Neurocognitive deficits in decision-making and planning of patients with DSM-III-R borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med. 2002, 32 (8): 1395-1405.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Jollant F, Bellivier F, Leboyer M, Astruc B, Torres S, Verdier R, Castelnau D, Malafosse A, Courtet P: Impaired decision making in suicide attempters. Am J Psychiat. 2005, 162 (2): 304-310. 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.2.304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Glicksohn J, Naor-Ziv R, Leshem R: Impulsive decision-making: Learning to gamble wisely?. Cognition. 2007, 105 (1): 195-205. 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.08.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Goudriann AE, Grekin ER, Sher KJ: Decision making and binge drinking: a longitudinal study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007, 31 (6): 928-938. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00378.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Kertzman S, Lidogoster H, Aizer A, Kotler M, Dannon PN: Risk-taking decisions in pathological gamblers is not a result of their impaired inhibition ability. Psychiat Res. 2011, 188 (1): 71-77. 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.02.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Petry NM: Substance abuse, pathological gambling, and impulsiveness. Drug Alcohol Depen. 2001, 63 (1): 29-38. 10.1016/S0376-8716(00)00188-5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Bosello R, Favaro A, Zanetti T, Soave M, Vidotto G, Huon G, Santonastaso P: Tattoos and piercings in adolescents: family conflicts and temperament. Riv Psichiatr. 2010, 45 (2): 102-106.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Xiao L, Bechara A, Cen S, Grenard J, Stacy A, Gallaher P, Wei Y, Jia Y, Anderson Johnson C: Affective decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in 10th-grade Chinese adolescent smokers. Nicotine Tob Res. 2008, 10 (6): 1085-1097. 10.1080/14622200802097530.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Hogarth L, Stillwell D, Tunney R: BIS impulsivity and acute nicotine exposure are associated with discounting global consequences in the Harvard game. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2013, 28 (1): 72-79. 10.1002/hup.2285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Reavis R, Overman WH: Adult sex differences on a decision-making task previously shown to depend on the orbital prefrontal cortex. Behav Neurosci. 2001, 115 (1): 196-206.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Bechara A, Damasio A, Damasio H, Anderson W: Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994, 50 (1–3): 7-15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Patton J, Stanford S, Barratt E: Factor Structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995, 51 (6): 768-774. 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Stanford MS, Mathias CW, Dougherty DM, Lake SL, Anderson NE, Patton JH: Fifty years of the barratt impulsiveness scale: an update and review. Pers Indiv Differ. 2009, 47 (5): 385-395. 10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Ernst M, Bolla K, Mouratidis M, Contoreggi C, Matochik JA, Kurian V, Cadet JL, Kimes AS, London ED: Decision-making in a risk-taking task. A PET Study. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2002, 26 (5): 682-691. 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00414-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Tversky A, Kahneman D: Beliefs in the law of small numbers. Psychol Bull. 1971, 76 (2): 105-110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. McMurran M, Blair M, Egan V: An investigation of the correlations between aggression, impulsiveness, social problem-solving, and alcohol use. Aggressive Behav. 2002, 28 (6): 439-445. 10.1002/ab.80017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Miller E, Joseph S, Tudway J: Assessing the component structure of four self-report measures of impulsivity. Pers Indiv Differ. 2004, 37 (2): 349-358. 10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H: Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000, 123 (11): 2189-2202. 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Bechara A: Risky business: Emotion, decision-making, and addiction. J Gambl Stud. 2003, 19 (1): 23-51. 10.1023/A:1021223113233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Busemeyer JR, Stout JC: A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: decomposing performance on the Bechara gambling task. Psychol Assess. 2002, 14 (3): 253-262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Kvaavik E, Rise J: How do impulsivity and education relate to smoking initiation and cessation among young adults?. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012, 73 (5): 804-810.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Pitts SR, Leventhal AM: Associations of functional and dysfunctional impulsivity to smoking characteristics. J Addict Med. 2012, 6 (3): 226-232. 10.1097/ADM.0b013e31825e2a67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Terracciano A, Costa PT: Smoking and the five-factor model of personality. Addiction. 2004, 99 (4): 472-481. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00687.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Yakir A, Rigbi A, Kanyas K, Pollak Y, Kahana G, Karni O, Eitan R, Kertzman S, Lerer B: Why do young women smoke? III. Attention and impulsivity as neurocognitive predisposing factors. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007, 17 (5): 339-351. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.09.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Wohlrab S, Stahl J, Kappeler PM: Modifying the body: motivations for getting tattooed and pierced. Body Image. 2007, 4: 87-95. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.12.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Beauregard J: Embodied identities: College women’s perspectives on body piercing using developmental models of psychology (dissertation doctoral). 2001, George Mason University

    Google Scholar 

  74. Tate J, Shelton B: Personality correlates of tattooing and body piercing in a college sample: the kids are alright. Pers Indiv Differ. 2008, 45: 281-285. 10.1016/j.paid.2008.04.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Semion Kertzman.

Additional information

Competing interests

SK is an employee of Anima Scan Ltd. Neither financial nor material support was received from any external resource for this work.

Authors’ contributions

SK has built the design of the study and methods used for the study and the interpretation of results, drafted the manuscript and contributed to the final manuscript. AK recruited participants, conducted the interviews and contributed to the final manuscript. MV coded and analysed the data. Supervised the manuscript writing: RL and AW. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Semion Kertzman, Alex Kagan contributed equally to this work.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kertzman, S., Kagan, A., Vainder, M. et al. Interactions between risky decisions, impulsiveness and smoking in young tattooed women. BMC Psychiatry 13, 278 (2013). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-244X-13-278

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-244X-13-278

Keywords