Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Family structure and posttraumatic stress reactions: a longitudinal study using multilevel analyses

Abstract

Background

There is limited research on the relevance of family structures to the development and maintenance of posttraumatic stress following disasters. We longitudinally studied the effects of marital and parental statuses on posttraumatic stress reactions after the 2004 Southeast Asian tsunami and whether persons in the same households had more shared stress reactions than others.

Method

The study included a tourist population of 641 Norwegian adult citizens, many of them from families with children. We measured posttraumatic stress symptoms with the Impact of Event Scale-Revised at 6 months and 2 years post-disaster. Analyses included multilevel methods with mixed effects models.

Results

Results showed that neither marital nor parental status was significantly related to posttraumatic stress. At both assessments, adults living in the same household reported levels of posttraumatic stress that were more similar to one another than adults who were not living together. Between households, disaster experiences were closely related to the variance in posttraumatic stress symptom levels at both assessments. Within households, however, disaster experiences were less related to the variance in symptom level at 2 years than at 6 months.

Conclusions

These results indicate that adult household members may influence one another's posttraumatic stress reactions as well as their interpretations of the disaster experiences over time. Our findings suggest that multilevel methods may provide important information about family processes after disasters.

Peer Review reports

Background

There has been increasing interest in the relevance of family factors to the development of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). In particular, family functioning and intrafamily support have been considered important [1–7]. Singles may receive less family support than married persons due to lack of partner [8]. However, the impact of marital status on posttraumatic stress is ambiguous. Some studies have found married individuals to have less posttraumatic stress reactions than unmarried individuals [4, 9, 10], while others have found the opposite effect [11] or no relationship between the variables [12–14]. Whereas one study found that divorced, separated, or widowed adults are at higher risk for PTSD than people who are presently married [15], another study found that this risk disappeared when controlling for other sociodemographic factors and trauma categories [16].

Few post-disaster studies have examined the effect of parental status on the development of PTSD. Parenthood may influence the risk of developing posttraumatic stress reactions through processes occurring both in the acute disaster situation [17] and post-disaster. Such an effect would be in accordance with classical developmental theories of bidirectional processes between parents and children [18, 19] as well as with more contemporary developmental theories [20, 21]. However, whereas several studies have found parental factors to be related to children's development of PTSD after disasters [22–25], relatively few longitudinal studies have investigated how parents are influenced by their children's level of posttraumatic stress reactions, and these studies have yielded discrepant results [26–29]. Furthermore, few studies have investigated whether having children relates to levels of posttraumatic stress. Studies have found that parents had higher levels of posttraumatic stress than nonparents after the Chernobyl disaster [30], after floods [8, 14], and after the 9/11 attacks [31]. The effect was particularly pronounced for single parents impacted by the 9/11 attacks [32]. However, other work suggest that parenthood or being in the company of children were not risk factors for posttraumatic stress reactions after the 2004 tsunami [10, 12].

An alternative method of investigating the relevance of family factors to posttraumatic stress reactions is by examining similarities in reactions within the family. We found only three studies that looked at the similarities of couples' reactions to disasters [14, 33, 34]. All three studies found general mental health or depression to be more similar within couples than for non-related adults but did not measure specific posttraumatic stress reactions. Two other studies found child siblings' posttraumatic stress reactions not to be more similar than other children's reactions [35, 36], thus it is unclear if family members do actually have more similar reactions after disaster than other disaster victims. If more than one person from a family is included in a study, the participants' responses are not independent of each other. Such grouping effects may influence results [37]; therefore, it has been suggested that disaster research should take grouping into account [38]. Multilevel analysis, including mixed effect models, is such a statistical method. It takes into account that some participants come from same subgroup, and thus for example analyze both the variability between individuals and between families [37]. However, very few disaster studies have taken into account the mutual experiences and shared reactions of families when analyzing predictors of posttraumatic stress reactions [10, 14, 39, 40]. Some resolve the problem by investigating only one participant from each household [7, 31] or by using sampling weights to correct for selection bias related to number of household members [32]. Others make no adjustments to account for participants from the same household [12, 33]. Thus, the question remains to what extent adult participants living in same household do have more similar reactions than other participants, and thereby are not independent observations, and how such possible grouping effects should be taken into account. This is important because the assumption of independent observations is one of the basic assumptions in standard statistical analyses.

Present study

This longitudinal study investigated posttraumatic stress reactions in Norwegian adults who experienced the tsunami as tourists in Southeast Asia on December 26, 2004. To our knowledge, this is the first longitudinal study of posttraumatic stress reactions to account for the multilevel effect of mutual households or families within the sample. We aimed to examine the relevance of family structures for adults' risk of posttraumatic stress reactions using two strategies: (1) by analyzing family structures as predictors for posttraumatic stress reactions and (2) by investigating possible similarities in reactions within families. The specific aims of the study were as follows:

To investigate differences in posttraumatic stress reactions between married participants and non-married participants

To investigate differences in posttraumatic stress reactions between parents and adults without children

To investigate, via multilevel analyses, whether adults within shared households had more similar posttraumatic stress reactions than adults from different households

Methods

Procedure

Shortly after the 2004 tsunami in Southeast Asia, Norwegian nationals who were evacuated from the disaster-stricken area were registered upon arrival in Norway. A postal questionnaire was sent to all registered persons 18 years or older (N = 2468) at 6 (T1) and 24 (T2) months post-tsunami. The questionnaire at T1 included questions concerning exposure, posttraumatic stress reactions, marital and parental status, and other background variables [41]. The questionnaire at T2 included questions about posttraumatic stress reactions [42]. Participants with the same address were assumed to be living in a common household. The study was approved by the Norwegian Social Science Data Services and The Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics.

Participants

While 868 and 1170 responded at T1 or T2 respectively, we received questionnaires for both T1 and T2 from 657 respondents. Five of these were excluded due to high levels of missing data on measures of posttraumatic stress reactions, and eleven more were excluded due to missing addresses. Therefore, the final sample included 641 participants.

At T1, 61.8% of the 641 participants had more than 12 years of education, and 75.5% were employed. There were multiple participants from the same household in 221 cases (34.5%). A total of 48.4% of the participants reported that they had traveled with a spouse or cohabitating partner. A total of 247 (38.5%) participants reported having children under the age of 18 years at T1; 240 participants (40.7%) reported to have responsibility for a child at time of the disaster, including 25 (4.2%) who had sole responsibility; and 310 (48.4%) reported to have traveled together with their own child, stepchild or foster child. At T1, 70.5% were married or cohabitating, 9.4% were no longer married and 20.1% were single. A total of 50 participants changed marital status from T1 to T2, 27 of which were no longer married or cohabitating, and 23 participants became married/cohabiter. More descriptive information about the participants is included in Table 1.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of the major study variables (N = 641)

Non-responders at T1 were more likely than responders to have resided in less severely affected locations in Southeast Asia [41] and were more often men; however, they were similar in age to responders [43]. The most frequently reported reasons for not participating were lack of interest or time, followed by lack of relevant experiences [44]. The final sample did not differ from responders who were excluded from the analyses in family features (marital status, proportion who had children at T1, or proportion of participants from same household) or posttraumatic stress reactions at T1. However, the excluded responders reported a lower average level of posttraumatic stress reactions at T2 than the analyzed sample (M(SD) excluded = 0.85 (0.82), M included = 1.05 (0.83), t(1396) = 2.89, p = .004).

Measures

Exposure and immediate response to the disaster

Based on earlier work [45], questions regarding a broad spectrum of tsunami experiences were assessed in the questionnaire 6-months post-tsunami. Based on earlier evaluations of the exposure experiences as risk factors [41], four questions were included in the present study to measure exposure: whether a participant had witnessed multiple dead bodies, had witnessed abandoned children, had been caught, touched or chased by waves, or had experienced the death of a family member or friend. Each question was answered no (0) or yes (1). Two questions were used to assess immediate subjective response to the disaster: fear, and feelings of helplessness, with both items rated on a five-point scale (0 = not at all, 1 = little, 2 = moderate, 3 = intense, 4 = extreme). These two items represented immediate response to the disaster, corresponding to the A2 criterion for PTSD from the DSM-IV [46].

Posttraumatic stress reactions

The Impact of Event Scale - Revised (IES-R) [47] was included at both assessments to measure the level of posttraumatic stress reactions during the previous two weeks. The IES-R includes 22 items with five response alternatives (0 = not at all, 1 = little, 2 = moderately, 3 = quite a bit, 4 = extremely). Total mean scores were based on all items. The psychometric properties of the IES-R have been extensively evaluated and deemed acceptable, with internal consistency within subscales reported to be between .81 and .91, test-retest reliability to be between .52 and .86, and correlation with other measures of posttraumatic stress reactions to be between .53 and .57 [48]. Similar acceptable measures or reliability have been found in a Norwegian non-clinical sample [49]. The internal consistency was high in the present sample (Cronbach's α = .96 and .95 at T1 and T2, respectively).

Data analysis

We excluded participants who were missing more than four replies to questions about posttraumatic stress reactions. For the remaining participants, missing values for these variables were replaced using expectation maximization algorithms (EM algorithms), which took into account a participant's scores on items within the same symptom cluster, the scores of the other respondents, and the correlations between items [50]. Dropout analyses were done using χ2-tests for categorical data and student t-tests for continuous data.

Chi-square tests were used for bivariate analyses of grouped variables. The effects of marital status and parental status on posttraumatic stress reactions were first tested with univariate mixed effects models adjusted for exposure and immediate subjective distress during the disaster. The effect of single parenthood was tested with a mixed effects model with random effects for differences between household and for individuals within household. There were no random effects regressors. Fixed effects regressors included both marital status and parental status, and also adjustment for the six questions concerning exposure and immediate subjective distress during the disaster. Mixed effects models were also used to analyze whether household members reported more similar posttraumatic stress reactions than participants from different households. The effect of household was tested with two mixed effects models, first an unadjusted model without predictors and next with a model adjusted for exposure and immediate subjective distress during the disaster. In this way, all models controlled for participants who lived in the same household. This multilevel approach means that the regression model has error terms at two levels, the individual level and the household level.

Similarity between household members is presented by intra-class correlations (ICC) computed from the random effects between and within household in the mixed effects models. ICC was calculated by dividing unexplained variance between households by the sum of unexplained variance between households and between individuals within same household. ICC can vary between 0 and 1. An ICC close to 0 indicates that household members are no more similar than other participants, whereas an ICC of 1 indicates that household members have identical responses. Confidence intervals for ICC were based on parametric bootstrapping and computed as bootstrap percentile intervals using 10,000 bootstrap replications. Bootstrapping is a general procedure that e.g. makes it possible to compute confidence intervals in cases where other methods are not easily available [51].

All tests were two-tailed, with a significance level of p ≤ .05. Statistical analyses were performed using PASW Statistics, version 18, and R, version 2.10.1, with packages nlme and boot.

Results

Marital status and parenthood

Marital status at T1 was not related to the level of posttraumatic stress reactions at T1 or at T2. The mean values (SD) of IES-R at T1 and T2, respectively, were 1.1 (0.8) and 1.0 (0.8) for married/cohabiters, 1.1 (0.8) and 1.1 (1.0) for single persons who had been previously married, and 1.1 (0.9) and 1.0 (0.8) for single persons who had not been previously married, corresponding to an average response close to little on the 0-4 scale (F(2, 515.2) = 0.02, p = .98 at T1 and F(2, 512.5) = 1.74, p = .18 at T2).

Furthermore, parents at T1 did not differ from non-parents in their level of posttraumatic stress reactions at T1 or at T2. The mean values (SD) of IES-R at T1 and T2, respectively, were 1.2 (0.9) and 1.1 (0.8) for non-parents and 1.1 (0.8) and 1.0 (0.8) for parents (t(503.8) = 1.77, p = .08, b(95% CI) = 0.10 (-0.01, 0.22) at T1 and t(469.4) = 0.91, p = .36, b(95% CI) = -0.06 (-0.07, 0.18) at T2).

To examine whether posttraumatic stress reactions differed between single parents and couples with children, marital status (single, divorced, separated, or widowed versus couple), parenthood (having a child versus not having a child), and the interaction effect between marital status and parenthood, were entered simultaneously into mixed effects models. No significant main effects or interaction effects on posttraumatic stress reactions were found at either of the two study times.

Further analyses were conducted to investigate whether parents and non-parents differed in their exposure or immediate emotional reactions during the tsunami. Compared to non-parents, parents did not witness more abandoned children, were not taken more often by the waves, and did not feel more helplessness during the tsunami. However, parents were less likely to have witnessed multiple dead bodies (χ2(1, N = 587) = 5.38, p = .02), were more likely to have lost family or friends in the tsunami (χ2(1, N = 641) = 8.97, p = .003), and felt more fear during the tsunami (t(505.2) = 2.39, p = .02, b(95% CI) = 0.28 (0.05, 0.52)).

Mutual household

At both time points, adults from the same household reported more similar levels of posttraumatic stress reactions than adults from different households. The unadjusted intra-class correlation (ICC) for posttraumatic stress reactions in the mixed effects model was .53 at T1 and .47 at T2 (Table 2). The confidence intervals for ICC at both times were sufficiently far from zero to indicate a considerable effect of mutual household. To examine whether similarities between members of the same household could be due to a greater number of shared experiences during the tsunami, we performed mixed effects models adjusted for the six questions concerning exposure and immediate emotional responses. Similar results were found, with ICC of .56 and .35 at T1 and T2, respectively (Table 2).

Table 2 Intra-class correlations for posttraumatic stress reactions, with and without adjustments for predictors at 6 (T1) and 24 (T2) months post-tsunami, and differences between these times

The decrease in unadjusted and adjusted ICC from T1 to T2 was not significant. However, the decrease in adjusted ICC was close to significant (Table 2). This decrease was related to changes in how much tsunami experiences explained variance in posttraumatic stress reactions between individuals within families. Taking into account the disaster experiences, the unexplained variances at T1 were reduced both at the individual level (36.0%) and at the household level (44.8%). At T2, unexplained variance between households had decreased (48.4%), whereas unexplained variance between individuals within the same household had decreased less when taking into account the disaster experiences (9.0%) (Table 2). Thus, tsunami experiences were still related to posttraumatic stress reactions of families at T2, but not as much to the reactions of individuals within families.

Discussion

In the present study, neither marital status nor parental status was related to the level of posttraumatic stress. Adults living in the same household had more similar levels of posttraumatic stress than adults not living together. The association between household members with regard to posttraumatic stress did not change from T1 to T2. Disaster experiences were associated with posttraumatic stress of individuals within families at T1, but there was almost no such association at T2. Thus, the impact of each family member's original disaster experiences on the level of posttraumatic stress decreased over time.

Neither single nor married parents had higher levels of posttraumatic stress reactions than adults without children. This is in accordance with findings in a similar study of Swedish tourists during the tsunami [10] and a study of Sri Lankan adults who were displaced after the tsunami [12]. However, other studies have found parenthood to be related to higher levels of reactions after a nuclear accident [30], floods [8, 14], and the 9/11 attack [31]. One possible reason for the discrepant findings is that our study, like the Swedish tsunami study, examined disaster survivors who were repatriated to stable home societies; thus, parents had fewer post-tsunami worries about their children's wellbeing and future. The fact that parents experienced both more fear than nonparents and were less likely to have witnessed dead bodies may have influenced our results as well. While the parents may have been more anxious because they worried about the wellbeing of their children, they may also have protected their children and thus also themselves from witness experiences. Thus, the findings indicate that it is possible that having children may both be related to factors enhancing and factors decreasing the risk of posttraumatic stress reactions [17], with such effects possibly nullifying each other.

Marital status was not related to an elevated or reduced level of posttraumatic stress reactions. This was surprising as singles may get less family support, and social support has been found to be a major protective factor against posttraumatic stress reactions [52, 53]. However, adults living in the same household had more similar levels of posttraumatic stress reactions than adults not living in the same household, with ICC indicating that approximately half of the variation in posttraumatic stress reactions was related to differences between adults within the same household. This is consistent with theories indicating that humans in relationships influence each other and often have converging interpretations of mutual experiences [54–56]. In most natural disasters, all members of a family are exposed, and in most instances, the members of the family live together after the disaster. Therefore, not only do family members have more resemblance in their disaster experiences than unrelated people, but they will also influence each other's coping strategies, recollections, interpretations and post-disaster reactions. However, child siblings have been found to have unrelated posttraumatic stress reactions, both in the same population as the present sample and after common disaster experiences during an earthquake [35, 36]. It is unclear why adult family members' reactions should be more similar than the reactions of child siblings. The difference between couples and child siblings may be age related and/or role related. Children are developmentally different from adults in how they are aware of their surroundings and their internal experiences [57]. It is also possible that couples communicate more and listen more to each other about the traumatic event and later reactions than siblings because of their cooperating role, especially their parental role. Whereas siblings may actively disagree, parents may seek cooperation and converging interpretation with their partner and co-caregiver.

We did not find any increase in household concordance of posttraumatic stress reactions from T1 to T2. This indicates that most of the family converging processes happened within six months of the disaster. However, the experiences during the disaster were less related to the posttraumatic stress reactions of the individuals within households at follow up than at the first assessment. These results indicate that family members influence each other's interpretations of the disaster over time. Thus, individual differences in interpretation of and reaction to the disaster diminish over time. The results thus indicate that over time, an individual's posttraumatic stress reactions may be influenced more by family members' interpretations and memories of the trauma than by actual exposure during the disaster.

Methodological considerations

This study had some methodological advantages. Almost all Norwegians who were tourists in the disaster area were invited to participate, reducing sample selection bias. The participants experienced a single, easily identifiable trauma and were largely protected from secondary adversities because they were able to quickly return to unaffected home communities. The tsunami-related processes between the persons in the household should therefore be less influenced by processes outside the household.

There was a relatively low response rate. However, due to the directionality of the dropout bias, the included participants seem to represent most of the heavily exposed Norwegian tourists in the tsunami-stricken areas [44].

The information was gathered by the use of postal questionnaires. Thus, participants from same household may have interacted during the filling out process. This may have influenced the results.

This study is limited to symptoms of posttraumatic stress. Other consequences of the disaster exposure, such as depression or functional impairment, were not analyzed or discussed. Children's levels of posttraumatic stress reactions were not controlled for in the present analyses, even though parent's and children's posttraumatic stress reactions are often related [58]. Other traumatic experiences were not taken into account [59].

The present article has evaluated marital status at first assessment as a risk factor for later posttraumatic stress reactions. Additional analyses indicated that change in marital status was not related to level of posttraumatic stress reactions (results not shown). Additional analyses (not shown) did also find that neither traveling with children nor having responsibility for children during the tsunami were related to level of posttraumatic stress reactions.

Conclusions

Adults living in the same household reported similar posttraumatic stress reactions. In addition, family members' interpretations of the disaster seemed to merge over time. This may be positive if the family moves in a favorable direction, but it indicates that for individuals who are not improving from posttraumatic stress reactions, it is important to investigate how their family interprets and perhaps contributes to the non-improving mental health. This study emphasizes the importance of a family-centered care that takes on an ecological grounded perspective when treating adults with posttraumatic stress reactions after common disaster experiences.

Methods of analysis that take into account the grouping of stress reactions within households provided valuable information about possible family processes. The study thus supports the importance of taking group levels into account when analyzing and discussing findings from studies including more than one participant from same household [38].

References

  1. McFarlane AC: Family functioning and overprotection following a natural disaster: The longitudinal effects of post-traumatic morbidity. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 1987, 21: 210-218. 10.3109/00048678709160914.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Norris FH, Uhl GA: Chronic stress as a mediator of acute stress: The case of Hurricane Hugo. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1993, 23 (16): 1263-1284. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01032.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Catapano F, Malafronte R, Lepre F, Cozzolino P, Arnone R, Lorenzo E, Tartaglia G, Starace F, Magliano L, Maj M: Psychological consequences of the 1998 landslide in Sarno, Italy: A community study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2001, 104 (6): 438-442. 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2001.00512.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Fullerton CS, Ursano RJ, Kao TC, Bharitya VR: Disaster-related bereavement: Acute symptoms and subsequent depression. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine. 1999, 70 (9): 902-909.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Wickrama KAS, Wickrama KAT: Family context of mental health risk in tsunami affected mothers: Findings from a pilot study in Sri Lanka. Social Science & Medicine. 2008, 66 (4): 994-1007. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.012.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Schwarz ED, Kowalski JM: Malignant memories. Effect of a shooting in the workplace on school personnel's attitudes. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 1993, 8 (4): 468-485. 10.1177/088626093008004003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Kaniasty K, Norris FH: Longitudinal linkages between perceived social support and posttraumatic stress symptoms: Sequential roles of social causation and social selection. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2008, 21 (3): 274-281. 10.1002/jts.20334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Solomon SD, Bravo M, Rubio-Stipec M, Canino G: Effect of family role on response to disaster. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1993, 6 (2): 255-269. 10.1002/jts.2490060208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Ursano RJ, Fullerton CS, Kao TC, Bhartiya VR: Longitudinal assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder and depression after exposure to traumatic death. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1995, 183 (1): 36-42. 10.1097/00005053-199501000-00007.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Wahlström L, Michelsen H, Schulman A, Backheden M: Different types of exposure to the 2004 tsunami are associated with different levels of psychological distress and posttraumatic stress. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2008, 21 (5): 463-470. 10.1002/jts.20360.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Brooks N, McKinlay W: Mental health consequences of the Lockerbie disaster. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1992, 5 (4): 527-543. 10.1002/jts.2490050403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Ranasinghe PD, Levy BR: Prevalence of and sex disparities in posttraumatic stress disorder in an internally displaced Sri Lankan population 6 months after the 2004 Tsunami. Disaster Medicine and Public Health Preparedness. 2007, 1 (1): 34-41. 10.1097/DMP.0b013e318068fbb7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Hollifield M, Hewage C, Gunawardena CN, Kodituwakku P, Bopagoda K, Weerarathnege K: Symptoms and coping in Sri Lanka 20-21 months after the 2004 tsunami. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2008, 192 (1): 39-44. 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.038422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Gleser GC, Green BL, Winget C: Prolonged psychosocial effects of disaster: A study of Buffalo Creek. 1981, New York, NY: Academic Press Inc

    Google Scholar 

  15. Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB: Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1995, 52 (12): 1048-1060. 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Breslau N, Peterson EL, Poisson LM, Schultz LR, Lucia VC: Estimating post-traumatic stress disorder in the community: Lifetime perspective and the impact of typical traumatic events. Psychological Medicine. 2004, 34 (5): 889-898. 10.1017/S0033291703001612.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Weisæth L: Collective traumatic stress: Crisis and catastrophes. Stress in health and disease. Edited by: Arnetz BB, Ekman R. 2006, Weinheim, Germany: WILEY-VCH Verlag GmbH & Co. KGaA

    Google Scholar 

  18. Lerner RM: Concepts and theories of human development. 1986, New York, NY: Newbery Award Records, Inc, 2

    Google Scholar 

  19. Stern DN: The interpersonal world of the infant. A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. 1985, New York, NY: Basic Books Inc

    Google Scholar 

  20. Siegel DJ: The developing mind. Toward a neurobiology of interpersonal experience. 1999, New York, NY: The Guilford Press

    Google Scholar 

  21. Berk LE: Child development. 2009, Boston, MA: Pearson, 8

    Google Scholar 

  22. Bloch DA, Silber E, Perry SE: Some factors in the emotional reaction of children to disaster. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1956, 113 (5): 416-422.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. McFarlane AC: Posttraumatic phenomena in a longitudinal study of children following a natural disaster. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1987, 26 (5): 764-769. 10.1097/00004583-198709000-00025.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Green BL, Korol M, Grace MC, Vary MJ, Leonard AC, Gleser EC, Smithson-Cohen S: Children and disaster: Age, gender and parental effects on PTSD symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1991, 30: 945-951. 10.1097/00004583-199111000-00012.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Catani C, Jacob N, Schauer E, Kohila M, Neuner F: Family violence, war, and natural disasters: A study of the effect of extreme stress on children's mental health in Sri Lanka. BMC Psychiatry. 2008, 8: 33-42. 10.1186/1471-244X-8-33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Koplewicz HS, Vogel JM, Solanto MV, Morrissey RF, Alonso CM, Abikoff H, Gallagher R, Novick RM: Child and parent response to the 1993 World Trade Center bombing. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2002, 15 (1): 77-85. 10.1023/A:1014339513128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Gershoff ET, Aber JL, Ware A, Kotler JA: Exposure to 9/11 among youth and their mothers in new york city: enduring associations with mental health and sociopolitical attitudes. Child Dev. 2010, 81 (4): 1142-1160. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01459.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Phillips D, Featherman DL, Jinyun L: Children as an evocative influence on adults' reactions in terrorism. Applied Developmental Science. 2004, 8 (4): 195-210. 10.1207/s1532480xads0804_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Kilmer RP, Gil-Rivas V: Responding to the needs of children and families after a disaster: Linkages between unmet needs and caregiver functioning. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2010, 80 (1): 135-142. 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01016.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Havenaar JM, Rumyantzeva GM, van den Brink W, Poelijoe NW, van den Bout J, van Engeland H, Koeter MW: Long-term mental health effects of the Chernobyl disaster: An epidemiologic survey in two former Soviet regions. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997, 154 (11): 1605-1607.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Stuber J, Fairbrother G, Galea S, Pfefferbaum B, Wilson-Genderson M, Vlahov D: Determinants of counseling for children in Manhattan after the September 11 attacks. Psychiatric Services. 2002, 53 (7): 815-822. 10.1176/appi.ps.53.7.815.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Stuber J, Resnick H, Galea S: Gender disparities in posttraumatic stress disorder after mass trauma. Gender Medicine. 2006, 3 (1): 54-67. 10.1016/S1550-8579(06)80194-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Vila G, Witkowski P, Tondini MC, Perez-Diaz F, Mouren-Simeoni MC, Jouvent R: A study of posttraumatic disorders in children who experienced an industrial disaster in the Briey region. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001, 10 (1): 10-18. 10.1007/s007870170042.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Kristensen P, Heir T, Herlofsen PH, Langsrud Ø, Weisæth L: Parental mental health after the accidental death of a son during military service: 23-year follow-up study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease.

  35. Asarnow J, Glynn S, Pynoos RS, Nahum J, Guthrie D, Cantwell DP, Franklin B: When the earth stops shaking: Earthquake sequelae among children diagnosed for pre-earthquake psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1999, 38 (8): 1016-1023. 10.1097/00004583-199908000-00018.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Nygaard E, Jensen TK, Dyb G: Posttraumatic stress reactions in siblings after mutual disaster: Relevance of family factors. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2010, 23 (2): 278-281.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Goldstein H: Multilevel mixed linear model analysis using iterative generalized least squares. Biometrika. 1986, 73 (1): 43-56. 10.1093/biomet/73.1.43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Norris FH: Disaster research methods: Past progress and future directions. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2006, 19 (2): 173-184. 10.1002/jts.20109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Dyb G, Jensen TK, Nygaard E: Children's and parents' posttraumatic stress reactions after the 2004 tsunami. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011, 16 (4): 621-634. 10.1177/1359104510391048.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Nomura Y, Chemtob CM: Effect of maternal psychopathology on behavioral problems in preschool children exposed to terrorism: Use of generalized estimating equations to integrate multiple informant reports. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicin. 2009, 163 (6): 531-539. 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Heir T, Weisæth L: Acute disaster exposure and mental health complaints of Norwegian tsunami survivors six months post disaster. Psychiatry. 2008, 71 (3): 266-276. 10.1521/psyc.2008.71.3.266.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Heir T, Piatigorsky A, Weisæth L: Longitudinal changes in recalled perceived life threat after a natural disaster. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2009, 194 (6): 510-514. 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.056580.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Heir T, Sandvik L, Weisæth L: Hallmarks of posttraumatic stress: Symptom Z-scores in a tsunami-affected tourist population. Psychopathology. 2009, 42 (3): 157-164. 10.1159/000207457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Hussain A, Weisaeth L, Heir T: Nonresponse to a population-based postdisaster postal questionnaire study. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2009, 22 (4): 324-328. 10.1002/jts.20431.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Weisæth L: The stressor-response relationship. Post-traumatic stress disorder. Edited by: Giller EL, Weisæth L. 1996, London: Bailliere Tindal, 191-216.

    Google Scholar 

  46. DSM-IV-TR: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 2000, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, text rev. edn, 4

    Google Scholar 

  47. Weiss DS, Marmar CR: The Impact of Event Scale-Revised. Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD. Edited by: Wilson JP, Keane TM. 1997, New York, NY: Guilford Press, 399-411.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Weiss DS: The Impact of Event Scale-Revised. Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD. Edited by: Wilson JP, Keane TM. 2004, New York, NY: The Guilford Press, 168-189.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Eid J, Larsson G, Johnsen BH, Laberg JC, Bartone PT, Carlstedt B: Psychometric properties of the Norwegian Impact of Event Scale-Revised in a non-clinical sample. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry. 2009, 1-7.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Schafer JL: Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. 1997, London, UK: Chapman & Hall

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  51. Efron B, Tibshirani R: Bootstrap methods for standard errors, confidence intervals, and other measures of statistical accuracy. Statistical Science. 1986, 1 (1): 54-77. 10.1214/ss/1177013815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Ozer EJ, Best SR, Lipsey TL, Weiss DS: Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2003, 129 (1): 52-73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Brewin CR, Andrews B, Valentine JD: Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000, 68 (5): 748-766.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Sherif M: The psychology of social norms. 1936, New York, NY: Harper

    Google Scholar 

  55. Asch SE: Social psychology. 1952, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  56. Nichols MP: Family therapy, concepts and methods. 2010, Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc, 9

    Google Scholar 

  57. Salmon K, Bryant RA: Posttaumatic stress disorder in children: The influence of developmental factors. Clinical Psychology Review. 2002, 22 (2): 163-188. 10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00086-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Alisic E, Jongmans MJ, van Wesel F, Kleber RJ: Building child trauma theory from longitudinal studies: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2011, 31 (5): 736-747. 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Neuner F, Schauer E, Catani C, Ruf M, Elbert T: Post-tsunami stress: A study of posttraumatic stress disorder in children living in three severely affected regions in Sri Lanka. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2006, 19 (3): 339-347. 10.1002/jts.20121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

The collection of the data was funded by The Research Council of Norway, and the analysis and writing of the report was funded by the Norwegian Centre for Violence and Traumatic Stress Studies. These institutions had no further role in analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Egil Nygaard.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

EN performed the statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript. TWL performed the statistical analysis and was involved in the revision of the manuscript. AH collected data and revised the manuscript. TH participated in the conception, design, and revision of the manuscript and was the general supervisor for the project. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nygaard, E., Wentzel-Larsen, T., Hussain, A. et al. Family structure and posttraumatic stress reactions: a longitudinal study using multilevel analyses. BMC Psychiatry 11, 195 (2011). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-244X-11-195

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-244X-11-195

Keywords